Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

download Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

of 5

Transcript of Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    1/10

    Sexual Selection, Paternal Care, and Concealed

    Ovulation in Humans

    Beverly I. Strassmann

    Changes in the social structure of early humans greatly

    enhanced the potential for paternal care to contribute

    to offspring success. Selection therefore favored females

    who mated with more paternal males. Since paternal

    care limits mating effort, males least successful as po-

    lygynists would have the most to gain by paternal be-

    havior, while the successful polygynists would gain least.

    Concealed ovulation may have evolved because it pro-

    moted the paternal tendencies of the less polygynous

    males who advanced female reproductive success the

    most. Since the offspring of indulgent males would have

    a competitive advantage over the offspring of more po-

    lygynous males, and females revealing the time of ovu-

    lation would become increasingly scarce, all males would

    eventually pursue a reproductive strategy emphasizing

    paternal effort over mating effort.

    In nonhuman primates the most polygynous males

    mate selectively with females who are likely to be ovu-

    lating. Sueh males would probably not mate with females

    having dhninished cues to the time of ovulation. The

    more paternally prone males could therefore consort

    with these females and experience a high confidence of

    paternity. Another characteristic of nonhuman primates

    is that the most successful polygynists tend to have a

    high dominance rank. Thus, female hominlds who mated

    with more paternal males may have sacrlflced having

    offspring with some of the gene advantages that con-

    tribute to dominance. However, subconscious physiolog-

    ical and psychological correlates of ovulation in humans

    may have tempted females to exploit infrequent, low-

    risk opportunities to mate outside the pair-bond with

    males of superior genetic fitness. These correlates may

    also promote conception irrespective of mating partner,

    or they may have helped females avoid rape.

    Key Words: Concealed ovulation; Dominance rank;

    Estrus; Paternal care; Polygyny; Reproductive stra-

    tegies; Sexual selection.

    Received April 3, 1980;accepted October 16, 1980

    Address

    reprint requests to: Beverly I. Strassmann, c/o

    Dept. of Entomology, Comstock Hall, Cornell University,

    Ithaca, NY 14853.

    INTRODUCTION

    In the superfamily Hominoidea, human females

    are unique in that they may be sexually receptive

    at any time of their cycle and do not exhibit a

    phase of pronounced physiological or behavioral

    estrous cues that overtly signal ovulation to

    themselves or to males (Butler, 1974; Alexander

    and Noonan, 1979; Benshoof and Thornhill,

    1979; Burley, 1979; Hrdy, 1979; Symons, 1979).

    Thus, in the evolutionary line leading to humans,

    there clearly has been selection to conceal ovu-

    lation. Understanding the selective background

    that led to the concealment of ovulation in hu-

    mans could improve our understanding of inter-

    actions between the sexes and provide insight

    helpful in improving contraceptive methods. The

    present paper will (1) review five recent hy-

    potheses on the evolution of concealed ovulation

    and (2) employ the theory of sexual selection to

    modify the scenario proposed by Alexander and

    Noonan (1979). The resulting hypothesis is sup-

    ported by behaviors of primates, and it is con-

    sistent with a possible explanation of the sub-

    conscious physiological and psychological

    correlates of human ovulation.

    PREVIOUS HYPOTHESES

    Burley (1979) proposes that concealed ovulation

    evolved to counteract a tendency of hominid fe-

    males to deliberately avoid conception. She ar-

    gues that the motivations for this behavior, such

    as fear of death, fear of pain, and the inconven-

    ience of having children, stem from the evolution

    of a large cerebrum. However, if a large cere-

    brum were responsible for such dramatically

    31

    Ethology and Sociobiology 2: 31-40 (1981)

    0 Elsevier North Holland, Inc.. 1981

    52 Vanderbilt Ave., New York, New York 10017

    0162-3095/81/0100031-10 02.50

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    2/10

    32 Beverly I. Strassmann

    nonadaptive behavior its disadvantages would

    probably have outweighed its advantages. More-

    over, studies of nonhuman primates make it

    doubtful that females in obvious estrus could

    have thwarted attempts by males to impregnate

    them. Selection for concealed ovulation as a

    means of promoting conception therefore would

    not have occurred. Burley’s conclusion that

    there is an apparent lack of female initiation of

    copulation during ovulation is contradicted by

    a study reporting an ovulatory peak in female-

    initiated sex (Adams et al., 1978). Adams et al.

    attribute the conflicting results of earlier studies

    (e.g., McCance et al., 1937; James, 1971; Spitz

    et al., 1975) to measures of sexual activity in

    which the ovulatory rise in female-initiated cop-

    ulations is masked by male-initiated copulations.

    Hrdy (1979) suggests that concealed ovula-

    tion evolved among prehominid females as a

    means of confusing paternity so that males

    would not be able to selectively kill the offspring

    of other males. This model may explain sham

    estrus in Hanuman langurs

    Presbyt is ~ntelus)

    but does not fit the evidence on prehominid so-

    cial structure. The primates from whom humans

    evolved probably experienced strong competi-

    tion between groups, thus enhancing unity and

    cooperation within groups. Selection for coop-

    erative hunting and predator defense would also

    have reinforced group solidarity (e.g., Alex-

    ander, 1974). If males in a troop gained by co-

    operating with each other they would not have

    profited from killing each other’s offspring. As

    Hrdy points out, in Savannah baboons (Papio

    c~nocephnlus) the males cooperate in predator

    defense and females have not evolved concealed

    ovulation. According to Hrdy, “any male who

    killed offspring belonging to a male still present

    in the troop would reduce the stake that male

    had in risking his life to defend the troop. Hence

    all infants in a troop containing an infanticidal

    male (including his own) would have been less

    likely to survive.” Moreover, in an affiliative

    troop, relatives and consorts of mothers would

    help them defend their infants from attacking

    males (Smuts, in preparation, cited in Hrdy,

    1979). Bygott (1979) asserts that exaggerated

    aggression over access to females will be pun-

    ished, since selfish males will not be able to par-

    ticipate in beneficial coalitions.

    Hrdy claims that continuous receptivity was

    part of the physiological heritage that prehom-

    inid females brought to the new lifestyle, distin-

    guishing humans from other primates. She may

    mean that concealed ovulation evolved in a com-

    mon ancestor to the Hominoidea (great apes and

    humans). At such an early stage in evolution,

    prehominids might not yet have become partic-

    ularly cooperative within groups. This idea is

    implausible since humans are the only members

    of the superfamily Hominoidea who have con-

    cealed ovulation.

    Even if cooperation within groups is strong,

    it could be argued that concealed ovulation

    evolved to counteract infanticide practiced by

    males from outside the group. Evidence provided

    by Bygott (1979) indicates that chimpanzees

    (Pan t roglod.y ty t rs) of the same troop have few

    aggressive encounters over females, while in-

    tergroup competition for females is fierce and

    sometimes involves infanticide. Similarly, infan-

    ticide among the Yanoama is often associated

    with the exchange of women in intercommunity

    warfare (Biocca, 1969). However, when infan-

    ticide is practiced on the infants already born to

    mothers recruited into a different group, males

    of the new troop may be quite certain they did

    not sire the infants. In this situation, the evo-

    lution of concealed ovulation would only benefit

    females who switched groups early in pregnancy

    before their condition could be detected by

    males. It is, therefore, not surprising that con-

    cealed ovulation has not evolved in chimpan-

    zees. These arguments imply that concealed

    ovulation evolved in humans for a reason other

    than intergroup infanticide. Such infanticide

    would, however, increase the importance of pa-

    ternal care; this could have been a precipitating

    factor in the evolution of concealed ovulation

    (Alexander and Noonan, 1979).

    Symons (1979) offers two scenarios to explain

    the loss of estrus in humans. In one, humans

    live in promiscuous polygynous bands and males

    give meat to females in exchange for sex. Ac-

    cording to Symons, this practice caused females

    to prolong their stage of sexual attractiveness.

    However, males who mated only with females

    in true estrus would have been selected for rel-

    ative to males who mated with females in either

    true or sham estrus. Unless one assumes that

    such discriminating males were extremely scarce.

    it is questionable whether females who pro-

    longed estrus would have been given more meat

    and thus been favored by selection.

    In another scenario, Symons (1979) and Ben-

    shoof and Thornhill (1979) suggest that con-

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    3/10

    Concealed Ovulation in Humans

    33

    cealed ovulation evolved to facilitate cuckoldry.

    The authors assert that females who did not ad-

    vertise ovulation would have minimized the ca-

    pacity of their mates to monitor their reproduc-

    tive cycles and guard them during ovulation. If

    at the time concealed ovulation evolved the

    human breeding system was monogamous or

    harem polygynous (and females sometimes had

    access to males who were genetically superior

    to their mates), the cuckoldry scenario is cred-

    ible. However, it is doubtful that females would

    evolve induced ovulation or another trait that

    would perfect their ability to cuckold males.

    Otherwise their mates would have such low con-

    fidence of paternity that selection would dim-

    inish their paternal care-whether or not males

    discovered infidelities.

    Alexander and Noonan’s hypothesis regard-

    ing the evolution of concealed ovulation is based

    on the following assumptions: (1) At the time

    ovulation became concealed human society was

    polygynous and consisted of multimale bands.

    Several studies link early humans with this social

    system (Lee and DeVore, 1968; Jolly, 1972;

    Alexander et al., 1979; Brace, 1979; Campbell,

    1979). (2) During this period in human evolution

    paternal care became an increasingly important

    determinant of offspring success. The increasing

    importance of paternal care, according to Alex-

    ander and Noonan, is associated with intergroup

    competition and the growth in group size and

    unity. These developments increased the com-

    plexity of social interactions and aggravated ju-

    venile disadvantages in experience and strength.

    Therefore, parents who conveyed to their off-

    spring the knowledge and skills needed for making

    the best decisions in complex social situations

    outreproduced less instructive parents. Other

    authors point out that conflicting selection for

    bipedality, which favors a narrow pelvis, and

    larger brain size, which favors a wide pelvis,

    could result in more altricial young whose brains

    would grow large after birth (e.g., Fox, 1972:

    Benshoof and Thornhill, 1979). The advantages

    of a longer learning period could also promote

    altriciality (Washburn’ and Lancaster, 1967:

    Pfeiffer, 1972). The increasing altriciality of

    human young would further enhance the advan-

    tages of parternal care.

    Alexander and Noonan’s two initial assump-

    tions, as well as their important idea that con-

    cealed ovulation evolved in the context of fe-

    males securing paternal care, are an integral part

    of the hypothesis advanced in this paper. Yet

    the evolutionary steps they propose are differ-

    ent. They suggest that through concealing ovu-

    lation females forced desirable males into con-

    sort relationships long enough to decrease the

    males’ chances of success in securing other mat-

    ings. The confidence of paternity of these males,

    according to Alexander and Noonan, was si-

    multaneously increased because potential com-

    petitors no longer knew when females were

    ovulating.

    Their hypothesis implies that the highest

    quality females would be in demand by the high-

    est quality males and would enter into prolonged

    consortships with these males before subordi-

    nate females and males would enter into con-

    sortships.’ It seems more likely that males most

    successful as polygynists wotdd be the last to

    increase their paternal care, not the first.

    In order for high ranking males to abandon

    their successful polygynous strategy and in-

    crease their paternal investment, differentials in

    female quality would have to be remarkably

    steep. The relevant sample sizes are small, but

    some studies of nonhuman primates seem to find

    instances in which females differ markedly in

    their attractiveness (Hausfater, 1975) or in their

    reproductive success (Goodall, 1971), while oth-

    ers point toward more uniform female quality

    (e.g., Loy, 1971; Kaufmann, 1965). Further

    studies are needed to determine whether steep

    differentials are likely to have existed among

    human females. In the absence of steep differ-

    entials, high ranking males who copulated around

    the most likely time of ovulation with females

    having a precise estrus would probably have the

    highest reproductive success.

    In addition to reducing the number of estrous

    females with whom a male can mate, partici-

    pation in a consortship potentially has other dis-

    ’ “The highest quality females fin terms of ability to bear

    and rear offspring, and, perhaps, to enhance their status or

    other correlates of reproductive success), would be in demand

    by the highest quality males most prone to polygyny; such

    females could afford to prolong estrus longer than others with-

    out risk of desertion by the male. Low quality females would

    be at least partially excluded from mating with the highest

    quality males by this ploy. Competition for matings by lower

    quality males would be reduced (because top quality males

    are already committed), making concealment of ovulation ad-

    vantageous to low ranking females in securing at least some

    parental care for their offspring” (Alexander and Noonan.

    1979, p. 448).

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    4/10

    34

    Beverly I. Strassmann

    advantages discussed by Hausfater (1975). He

    postulates such negative consequences as re-

    duced food intake resulting from the need for

    constant guarding of the female, and increased

    exposure to aggression and harassment from

    other males. In his study of yellow baboons

    (Pupio cynocephalus), the first ranking adult

    males mated and consorted primarily on the op-

    timal day for mating. Lower ranking adults con-

    sorted on as many potentially fertile cycle days

    as possible. According to Hausfater, the strategy

    of the first ranking males gives them a relatively

    high probability of impregnating females while

    minimizing the negative consequences of con-

    sortship.

    High ranking males who entered into con-

    sortships with females who prolonged estrus

    would suffer the additional disadvantage of the

    higher cost of high confidence of paternity.

    When males need to guard their mate only during

    the brief period of estrus, confidence of paternity

    is least expensive (Benshoof and Thornhill,

    1979; Burley, 1979; Symons, 1979). If a male

    guards a female continuously, he compromises

    his ability to hunt and carry out the other activ-

    ities that are at least indirectly an important com-

    ponent of paternal care. Although concealed

    ovulation would greatly reduce the probability

    of matings outside the pair-bond resulting in

    pregnancy, the following males would still at-

    tempt such matings: unmated males, males with

    pregnant mates, and other males as low cost

    opportunities arose. Since confidence of patern-

    ity would be more expensive for high-ranking

    males, it is unclear how concealed ovulation in

    Alexander and Noonan’s scheme could increase

    paternal investment. In view of the disadvan-

    tages of consortships to dominant males, it is

    necessary to add to Alexander and Noonan’s

    scenario the condition that the first males to pro-

    long their consortships would probably be those

    having a low probability of securing matings.

    Moreover, the females with whom such males

    consorted would have to be sufficiently unat-

    tractive to the polygynous males that their con-

    sorts would have a high confidence of paternity.

    CONCEALED OVULATION AND MATE

    SELECTION

    Due to anisogamy, which may be the ultimate

    source of differential parental investment be-

    tween the sexes, females are usually a limiting

    resource to males (Bateman, 1948; Trivers,

    1972). One consequence is a general tendency

    toward polygynous breeding systems in which

    there is great variance in reproductive success

    among males. In their competition to secure suc-

    cessful matings with females, conspecific males

    have evolved alternative strategies involving

    both behavioral and morphological differences.

    For a recent review of alternative male strategies

    among several classes of organisms see Cade

    (1979).

    This theoretical framework suggests a pos-

    sible selective background for the evolution of

    concealed ovulation. If humans were polygyn-

    ous at the time concealed ovulation evolved,

    some males by definition would have had more

    success at securing matings resulting in preg-

    nancy. Studies of nonhuman primates indicate

    that those males who secure the most matings

    around ovulation tend to be dominant while sub-

    ordinate males find seeking matings a costly en-

    deavor with a lower probability of success (see

    Conaway and Koford, 1965; DeVore, 1965:

    Kaufmann, 1965; Lindburg, 1971; Loy, 1971:

    Hausfater, 1975). Those males who are least suc-

    cessful as polygynists would tend to expend

    greater paternal effort on the offspring that they

    produced with a relatively limited number of fe-

    males. In fact, their options for increasing their

    reproductive success would resemble those of

    females. The successful polygynists, on the con-

    trary, would tend to expend less paternal effort

    on each of the offspring of a larger number of

    females. This argument is based on the assump-

    tion that time and energy spent on mating effort

    will limit paternal effort (see Alexander and Bor-

    gia, 1979). Borgia (1979) formulates the similar

    argument that males who are unlikely to be suc-

    cessful at securing matings because of their rel-

    atively low genetic fitness may enhance their

    reproductive success by providing females and

    their young with material benefits. Of course,

    greater paternal effort on the part of subordinate

    males would have to be associated with en-

    hanced confidence of paternity, and it would

    have to be the best strategy for these males to

    pursue in terms of their overall life history.

    As the potential for paternal care to be a de-

    terminant of offspring success increased so as

    to exceed the importance of the male’s genetic

    assets which do not contribute to paternal care,

    those males least able to monopolize matings

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    5/10

    Concealed Ovulation in Humans

    35

    would have become the most desirable mates.

    These arguments suggest that concealed ovula-

    tion evolved because it favored the strategy of

    the subordinate, more paternal males who pro-

    moted female reproductive success the most.

    Females sacrificed the genetic benefits offered

    by the high ranking males in exchange for the

    more important paternal benefits offered by

    lower ranking males. Borgia (1979) also arrives

    at the conclusion that females sometimes must

    compromise potential gain from either the ma-

    terial or the genetic benefits offered by males in

    order to maximize reproductive success.

    How would the concealment of ovulation en-

    hance the confidence of paternity of the subor-

    dinate males? Subordinate males could probably

    keep their consorts from copulating with other

    low ranking males, but not from the high ranking

    males. However, if females who concealed ovu-

    lation were sufficiently unattractive to the high

    ranking males, their lower ranking consorts

    would have a higher confidence of paternity.

    Female Attractivity

    Behaviors of nonhuman primates indicate that

    females who concealed ovulation by gradually

    dampening their estrus signs might have been

    unattractive to the dominant males. Observa-

    tions of baboons, rhesus monkeys, and chim-

    panzees reveal a strong degree of selectivity in

    the mating of the dominant males. First ranking

    male yellow baboons do not mate on all the po-

    tentially fertile days of a female’s cycle, but in-

    stead constrain their mating to the optimal day,

    sometimes even the optimal time of day (Haus-

    fater, 1975). On the optimal day estrus signs such

    as sexual swellings are most pronounced. In

    other studies of baboons (Papio w-sinus and P.

    am is)

    and macaques

    (Macaca mulatta),

    it was

    found that the more advanced the estrous con-

    dition of the female, the more exclusively was

    copulation confined to high ranking males (Hall,

    1962; DeVore and Hall, 1965). Moreover, there

    are some females with whom the dominant males

    do not consort at all. For example, Hausfater

    (1975) found that when three female yellow ba-

    boons were in estrus, two different first ranking

    males did not consort with them even if no other

    estrous females were present in the group.

    Goodall (1965, 1968) found that female chim-

    panzees during their first few sexual swellings

    appeared unattractive to mature males, but fre-

    quently solicited mounting from adolescents.

    Since dominant males cue in on estrus signs and

    do not mate with females with partial swellings,

    they might pass up opportunities to mate with

    females who had begun to reduce their estrus

    signs, even though these females would be fer-

    tile. This behavior would reflect an inability of

    the males to distinguish between females un-

    likely to be fertile and females concealing ovu-

    lation. Low ranking males would probably mate

    with females who dampened their estrus signs

    because they mate with females having partial

    swellings.

    It is probable that at the time concealed ovu-

    lation evolved all males including the dominant

    ones already wqre somewhat inclined to be pa-

    ternal-perhaps bnly through protecting past or

    present female c sorts or their young. Such

    behavior is thought TO occur to some extent in

    all the great apes (Mitchell, 1969) and would re-

    duce the time and energy available to the dom-

    inant males to seek suboptimal matings. Domi-

    nant males would be especially disinclined to

    attempt copulations with females appearing su-

    boptimal if these females were being vigorously

    defended by their low ranking male consorts.

    Since their matings with females appearing su-

    boptimal would be rare, dominant males would

    not experience strong selection for the ability to

    distinguish between females concealing ovula-

    tion and females unlikely to be fertile. The rare

    matings that would occur between dominant

    males and females concealing ovulation would

    not diminish the paternal behavior of the fe-

    males’ indulgent consorts because the latter

    would have been copulating with the females

    much more regularly and therefore would be

    more likely to sire offspring.

    In nonhumans there is a close correlation

    between female attractivity and estrogen levels

    (Beach, 1976; Baum et al., 1977). Hausfater

    (1975) suggests that in yellow baboons a sexual

    pheromone may be the mechanism governing

    cycle day and individual selectivity. If so, it is

    unlikely that a female who decreased her non-

    hormonal estrus signs but retained her hormonal

    estrus signs would be unattractive to dominant

    males. Since estrogen is an agent in ovulation,

    it is necessarily present in high concentration

    during ovulation. Therefore, in the loss of estrus.

    the hormonal cues to ovulation could not have

    been obscured through a reduction in hormone

    production. Symons (1979) suggests that the loss

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    6/10

    36 Beverly I. Strassmann

    of estrus may have begun with the continuous

    production of estrogen, or a pheromone based

    on estrogen. Another possibility is that there was

    a slight alteration in an estrogen based phero-

    mone that, in association with the dampened

    visual signs, deterred high ranking males from

    mating with females concealing ovulation. Se-

    lection for males to detect the new pheromone

    and associate it with ovulation would be espe-

    cially weak if it varied among females. The vol-

    atile organic constituents of the vaginal secre-

    tions of modern humans have been shown to

    vary (Preti and Huggins. 1975). The visual signs

    of ovulation may have been lost through a neo-

    tenous reduction in the sexual skin or in the

    sensitivity of the sexual skin to estrogen.

    Extended Sham Estrus Versus Direct Loss of

    Estrus

    It is doubtful that females concealed ovulation

    by extending the duration of estrus signs, making

    sham estrus indistinguishable from true estrus,

    as has been suggested (Alexander and Noonan,

    1979: Burley, 1979; Symons, 1979). This ploy

    would have been less likely to allow females to

    grant paternal males a high confidence of pa-

    ternity than would the concealment of ovulation

    through the direct loss or disguisement of es-

    trous cues. One could expect the most success-

    ful polygynists not to copulate with females who

    extended their signs because the longer period

    of consortship necessary to give a high proba-

    bility of conception would reduce the number

    of less costly matings these males could secure.

    However, females in prolonged sham estrus

    would occasionally be exposed to high ranking

    males from outside their group. Hrdy (1977,

    1979) observed that female langurs in sham es-

    trus who are ignored by resident males are often

    sought after by extra-troop males. She attributes

    this behavioral discrepancy to differences in the

    ability of resident and nonresident males to mon-

    itor females’ cycles, and thereby distinguish

    pseudo-estrus from true estrus. Thus, polygyn-

    ous males from outside the’band would probably

    attempt copulations with hominid females in

    sham estrus. Such males would compromise the

    confidence of paternity of the paternal males

    who mated with females who concealed ovula-

    tion through sham estrus. Moreover, extension

    of such estrous cues as sexual swellings would

    probably impose on females a great energetic

    expense. Thus, ovulation was probably con-

    cealed through the direct dampening of its visual

    signs and the masking of its hormonal signs.

    The Diversion of Mating Effort into Paternal

    Effort

    Since the offspring of indulgent males would re-

    ceive the most paternal care, they would have

    a competitive advantage over the offspring of

    more polygynous males. Compared to the pa-

    ternal strategy, the polygynous strategy would

    become increasingly less successful. Females

    who concealed ovulation would, on the average,

    have more indulgent mates and achieve greater

    reproductive success than females who adver-

    tised ovulation. Eventually, females with a con-

    spicuous estrus would no longer exist, nor would

    there be males exploiting this physiology by em-

    phasizing mating effort over paternal effort. Fe-

    male willingness to mate would gradually have

    extended throughout the menstrual cycle be-

    cause as long as receptivity was keyed to the

    ovulatory phase ovulation would not have been

    concealed.

    Polygynous behavior would not disappear,

    but each male would mate with fewer females

    on the average and would exhibit more paternal

    care. Several factors explain why polygynous

    behavior would not be lost entirely. First,

    greater access to resources that are a component

    of paternal care would enable some males to

    have more mates or seek more matings than

    would be the case for most males. Second, there

    are diminishing returns from paternal care after

    a certain level of indulgence. Third, although

    paternal care restricts mating effort overall, sit-

    uations would arise in which males could secure

    additional mates or copulations without invok-

    ing a disadvantageous trade-off with paternal

    care.

    Paternal Investment: Male Versus Female

    Advantages

    Burley (1979) has argued that concealed ovula-

    tion would not evolve to induce males into pro-

    viding paternal care because males would in-

    crease their paternal investment anyway when

    this behavior became advantageous for them.

    This assertion ignores the probable existence of

    a stage in human evolution in which increased

    paternal investment would have benefited fe-

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    7/10

    Concealed Ovulation in Humans

    7

    males greatly but would not yet have been

    strongly selected for among males. In a poly-

    gynous primate species, subordinate males would

    have had the most to gain by paternal behavior

    but their low confidence of paternity would have

    restricted the development of their paternal

    tendencies. It cannot be assumed that the high

    ranking males would have increased their pa-

    ternal investment since these males would al-

    ready have been achieving high reproductive

    success through a reproductive strategy empha-

    sizing mating effort. If these males increased

    their investment, time and energy constraints

    would then have reduced the total number of

    matings they secured, making increased invest-

    ment a marginal advantage at best. It could even

    have been disadvantageous because it would

    probably have increased the number of matings

    secured by lower ranking males or raised the

    confidence of paternity of lower ranking males,

    enabling them to be more paternal. Therefore,

    a female adaptation that increased the advan-

    tages of paternal investment to males could have

    been favorably selected.-The hypothesis ad-

    vanced in this paper is that concealed ovulation

    enhanced the advantages of paternal care to sub-

    ordinate males by increasing their confidence of

    paternity. Assuming that paternal care was a

    more important determinant of offspring success

    than the dominance status of the father, con-

    cealed oculation would have benefited females

    and therefore been positively selected.

    PREDICTIONS

    If concealed ovulation evolved because it in-

    creased the reproductive advantages that males

    could gain from paternal effort relative to mating

    effort, certain predictions should follow:

    If paternal care were sufficiently important

    in determining offspring success so that a

    male’s genes unrelated to paternal care were

    relatively trivial, then one would expect the

    ability of females to control conception to be

    relatively poorly developed and cuckoldry to

    be rare.

    If, however, genes irrelevant to paternal care

    were still significant in determining repro-

    ductive success, one would expect females

    to retain the ability to exploit occasional

    ow

    risk opportunities to mate outside the pair-

    bond with males of superior genetic fitness

    (Alexander, 1975).

    In either case, conscious knowledge of ovulation

    would be selected against because it would

    hinder the ability of females to deceive males

    and promote paternal care (see Alexander and

    Noonan, 1979; Benshoof and Thornhill, 1979).

    In the second case, there would be selected (or

    never lost in the first place) subconscious phys-

    iological and psychological correlates of ovula-

    tion that increase the probability that isolated

    matings with more fit males result in pregnancy.

    An example is facilitated sperm transport men-

    tioned by Benshoof and Thomhill (1979).

    PHYSIOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL

    CORRELATES OF OVULATION

    A review of the subject of ovulation reveals an

    array of physiological correlates. Concomitant

    with the various hormonal changes around ovu-

    lation are various changes in the cervical secre-

    tions that increase the ability of sperm to mi-

    grate. When coupled with sexual arousal in the

    female, they increase the probability of concep-

    tion (Levin and Wagner, 1977). Hormonal changes

    in the ovarian cycle of the human female have

    been shown to effect the CNS and cause changes

    in sensory stimuli. Diamond et al. (1972) found

    that visual sensitivity built up to a peak at mid-

    cycle and remained high until menstruation

    when it dropped precipitously. Women taking

    nonsequential oral contraceptives, and men

    tested by the same procedures, did not undergo

    significant visual threshold changes. Laws (1977)

    discovered that changes in the neuroendocrine

    system lower the acoustic reflex threshold of

    women using oral contraceptives or at day 16 in

    their menstrual cycle. Semesuk et al. (cited in

    Diamond et al., 1972) also found changes in

    acoustic sensitivity to be related to the menstrual

    cycle. Olfactory sensitivity of women for invo-

    latile compounds has been correlated with the

    menstrual cycle and estrogen production, with

    the greatest sensitivity occurring around ovu-

    lation (Le Magnen, 1950; Schneider et al., 1958;

    Good et al. 1976; Mair et al., 1978). Sensitivity

    to pain was found to decrease during ovulation

    by Buselli et al. (cited in Diamond et al., 1972).

    In addition to the physiological changes, a

    periodicity in psychological state is related to

    the ovarian cycle. Several studies show that

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    8/10

      8

    Beverly 1. Strassmann

    aggression, anxiety, inner-directed hostility, and

    fatigue are lowest at mid-cycle while libido is

    highest (McCance et al., 1937; Benedek and

    Rubenstein, 1939; Altmann et al., 1941: Hart,

    1960; Gottschalk et al., 1962; Moos, 1969:

    Luschen and Pierce. 1972). Other studies show

    a premenstrual or postmenstrual rise in con-

    scious sexual desire (e.g., Kinsey, 1953). The

    curve showing that sexual arousal peaks during

    ovulation found by Altmann et al. (1941) and

    Moos (1969) corresponded closely to a

    urve

    for

    visual sensitivity (Diamond et al., 1972). Adams

    et al. (1978) found an increase in female initiated

    sexual behavior during ovulation.

    Tests of the physiological and psychological

    correlates of ovulation in nonhuman primates

    would be helpful in determining whether these

    traits are vestigial, or attributes that selection

    has actively retained or even heightened in hu-

    mans. Of

    course, analysis of these correlates is

    complicated

    since one explanation could not ac-

    count for all of them. Beyond the idea that these

    correlates are simply vestigial remnants of es-

    trus, there are at least three possible explana-

    tions for their occurrence. The marked improve-

    ment in acoustic, visual, and olfactory sensitivity

    during ovulation may have improved the ability

    of females to avoid rapists operating by stealth

    or ambush. In the literature on ovulation, the

    correlates are widely regarded as a mechanism

    to increase the probability of conception (e.g.,

    Diamond et al., 1972). Diamond et al. assert that

    the psychological and sensory changes could in-

    crease the probability of coitus occurring during

    ovulation because of the combined effect of

    greater sensitivity to arousing stimuli and de-

    creased sensitivity to pain. A third possibility is

    that the psychological and sensory changes

    tempt females to mate outside the pair-bond with

    males having superior genes. This explanation

    supports the hypothesis that concealed ovula-

    tion evolved because it allowed females to pro-

    mote paternal care among the males most prone

    to it anyway, but who were not necessarily the

    optimal genetic fathers of their offspring. How-

    ever, matings outside the pair-bond would not

    be expected to be sufticiently successful that the

    paternal males would have a low confidence of

    paternity.

    I thank Richard Alexander for introducing the topic of

    concealed ovulation in one of his many interesting

    classroom lectures. This paper has benefited greatly

    from informative and discerning comments he offered

    on all drafts. I also appreciate the helpful suggestions

    of Gerald Borgia, Katharine Noonan, Donald Symons.

    and Randy Thornhill.

    Nancy

    Burley, Lee Benshoof,

    and Randy Thornhill obligingly sent me their prepub-

    lication manuscripts. Joan Strassmann provided much

    encouragement.

    REFERENCES

    ..______

    Adams, D.B., Gold, A.R. Burt, A.D. Rise in female

    initiated sexual activity at ovulation and its

    suppression by oral contraceptives. N~M, England

    Journal

    oJ‘Medic,ine 229(21): 1145-l 150 (1978).

    Alexander, R.D. The evolution of social behavior.

    Ann. Rev. of Ecol. and Syst. 5: 325-383 (1974).

    --. Natural selection and specialized chorusing be-

    havior in acoustical insects. In In.rects.

    Scietwe md

    Soc,iety,

    D. Pimentel (Ed.). Ithaca. NY: Cornell

    Univ. Press, 1975.

    -, Hoogland, J.L. Howard, R.D. Noonan. K.M.

    Sherman, P.W. Sexual dimorphisms and breeding

    systems in pinnipeds. ungulates. primates, and hu-

    mans. In Evolutioncrry Biology und Human Social

    Behavior: An Anfhropologiccrl Perspec,tive. N.A.

    Chagnon and W.G. Irons (Eds.). North Scituate,

    MA: Duxbury Press, 1979.

    -, Noonan, K.M. Concealment of ovulation. pa-

    rental care, and human social evolution. In Eva-

    lutionary Biology und Humon Sock/ Behavior: An

    Anthropological Perspective,

    N.A. Chagnon and

    W.G. Irons (Eds.). North Scituate. MA: Duxbury

    Press. 1979.

    -, Borgia, G. On the origin and basis of the male

    female phenomenon. In Se.rual

    Selection in fnsecrs,

    M.F. Blum and N.A. Blum (Eds.). New York:

    Adademic Press, 1979.

    Altmann, M., Knowles, E., Bull, H.D. A psychoso-

    matic study of the sex cycle in women. Psychosom.

    Med. 3: 199-225 (1941).

    Bateman, A.J. Intrasexual

    selection in Drosophila.

    Heredity 2: 349-368 (1948).

    Baum, M.J.. Everitt, B.J. Herbert, J. Keverne,

    E.B. Hormonal basis of proceptivity and receptiv-

    ity in female primates.

    Arc,hives of Sexuul Behavior

    6: 173-192 (1977).

    Beach, F.A. Sexual attractivity, proceptivity, and re-

    ceptivity in female mammals.

    Hormones und Be-

    huvior 7: 105-138 (1976).

    Benedek, T., Rubenstein. B.B. The correlations be-

    tween ovarian activity and psychodynamic pro-

    cesses: I.

    The ovulatory phase.

    Psychosom. Med.

    I :

    245-270 (1939).

    Benshoof, L., Thornhill, R. The evolution of monog-

    amy and concealed ovulation in humans. Journal

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    9/10

    Concealed Ovulation in Humans 39

    of Social and Bi ol ogical St ructur es 2: 95-106

    1979).

    Biocca, E. Yanoama. New York: E.P. Dutton Co.,

    1969.

    Borgia, G. Sexual selection and the evolution of mat-

    ing systems. In

    Sexual Sel ecti on and Reproducti ve

    Competi ti on in nsects, M.S. Blum and N.A. Blum

    (Eds.). New York: Academic Press, 1979.

    Brace, C.L. Biological parameters and Pleistocene

    hominid lifeways. In

    Primate Ecology and Human

    Or igi ns: Ecologi cal Inj luences on Social Organi-

    zation, I.S. Bernstein and E.O. Smith (Eds.). New

    York: Garland STPM Press, 1979.

    Burley, N. The evolution of concealed ovulation.

    Ameri can Na t ural ist 114 6): 835-858 1979).

    Butler, H. Evolutionary trends in primate sex cycles.

    Contri buti ons to Primat ology 3: 2-35 1974).

    Bygott, J.D. Agonistic behavior,‘dominance, and so-

    cial structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe

    National Park. In The Great Apes, B. Hamburg and

    E. McCown (Eds.). Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/

    Cummings, 1979.

    Cade, W. The evolution of alternative male reproduc-

    tive strategies in field crickets. In Sexual Selection

    and Reproductiv e Competi ti on in Insects, M.S.

    Blum and N.A. Bium (Eds.). New York: Academic

    Press, 1979.

    Campbell, B. Ecological factors and social organiza-

    tion in human evolution. In Primate Ecology and

    Human Or igins: Ecologi cal Inf luences on Social

    Organization,

    I.S. Bernstein and E.O. Smith (Eds.).

    New York: Garland STPM Press, 1979.

    Conaway, C.H., Koford, C.B. Estrous cycles and

    mating behavior in a free-ranging band of rhesus

    monkeys. Journal of Mammal ogy 45: 577-588

    1965).

    DeVore, I. Male dominance and mating behavior in

    baboons. In Sex and Behavior, F.A. Beach (Ed.).

    New York: Wiley, 1965.

    -, Hall, K.R.L. Baboon social behavior. In Pri -

    mat e Behavi or: Field St udi es

    o f

    Monkeys and

    Apes, I. DeVore (Ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart,

    and Winston, 1965.

    Diamond M., Diamond, A.L., Mast, M. Visual sen-

    sitivity and sexual arousal levels during the men-

    strual cycle.

    Journal

    o f

    Nervous and M ental

    Dis-

    ease 155(3): 170-176 (1972).

    Fox, R. Alliance and constraint: sexual selection and

    the evolution of human kinship systems. In Sexual

    Sel ecti on and t he Descent o f Man: 1871-1971, B.

    Campbell (Ed.). Chicago: Aldine, 1972.

    Good, P.R., Geary, N., Engen, T. The effect of es-

    trogen on odor detection.

    Chemical Senses and

    Flav or 2: 45-50 1976).

    Goodall, J. Chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream Re-

    serve. In

    Pri mat e Behavi or: Field St udi es

    o f

    Mon-

    keys and Apes, I. DeVore (Ed.). New York: Holt,

    Rinehart, and Winston, 1965.

    -. The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the

    Gombe Stream Reserve. Ani m. Behav. M onogr. 1

    Part 3: 161-311 (1968).

    -. In The Shadow o f Man. Boston: Houghton Mif-

    flin, 1971.

    Gottschalk, L.A., Kaplan, S.M. Gleser, G.C. Winget,

    C.M. Variations in magnitude of emotion: A method

    applied to anxiety and hostility during phases of the

    menstrual cycle.

    Psychosom. M ed. 24: 300-311

    1962).

    Hall, K.R.L. The sexual, agonistic and derived social

    behaviour patterns of the wild chacma baboon,

    Papi o ursinus. Pr oc. 2001. Society London 139 1 ):

    238-327 1962).

    Hart, R.D. Monthly rhythm of libido in married

    women. Bri t ish M edi cal Journal 1: 1023-1024

    1960).

    Hausfater, G. Dominance and reproduction in ba-

    boons Papio cynocephalus). Contri b. to Prima-

    tology 7. Basel: S. Karger, 1975.

    Hrdy, S. The Langurs of Abu: Femal e and M al e St ra-

    tegies o f Reproduction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

    Univ. Press, 1977.

    -. Infanticide among animals.

    Ethology and Socio-

    biology l(1): 13-40 (1979).

    James, W.H. The distribution of coitus within the

    human intermenstruam. J. Biosoc. Sci. 3: 159-171

    (1971).

    Jolly, A. The Evolution o f Primate Behavior . N ew

    York: Macmillan, Co., 1972.

    Kaufmann, J.H. A three-year study of mating behav-

    ior in a free-ranging band of rhesus monkeys.

    Ecol-

    ogy 46 4): 500-512 1965).

    Kinsey, A.C., and the Staff of the Institute for Sex

    Research. Sexual Behavi or in t he Human Femal e.

    Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders, 1953.

    Laws, D.W. The effects of the normal menstrual cycle

    and the use of oral contraceptives on the acoustic

    reflex threshold. Di sser. Abst . I nt er. 37 7-B): 3338

    1977).

    Lee, R.B., DeVore, I. (Eds.). Man the Hunter. Chi-

    cago: Aldine, 1968.

    Le Magnen, J. Nouvelles don&es sur le phtnomene

    de I’exaltolide. Comp tes Rendus de I ’A cademie des

    Sciences. Paris, 1950.

    Levin, R.J., Wagner, G. Human vaginal fluid ionic

    composition and modification by sexual arousal.

    Journal o f Physi ol ogy 266 l ): 62P-63P 1977).

    Lindburg, D.G. The rhesus monkey in north India: An

    ecological and behavioral study. In

    Primate Be-

    havior, L.A. Rosenblum (Ed.). New York: Aca-

    demic Press, 1971, Vol. 2.

    Loy, J. Estrous behavior of free-ranging rhesus mon-

    keys

    M ucaca mulat ta). Primat es 12 l ): l-31 1971).

    Luschen, M.E., Pierce, D.M. Effect of the menstrual

    cycle on mood and sexual arousability.

    J. Sex Res.

    8: 41-47 (1972).

    Mair, R.G., Bouffard, J.A. Engen, T. Morton,

  • 8/20/2019 Strassmann 1981 Ethol Sociobiol, Evol Ovulacion Oculta

    10/10

    Beverly 1. Strassmann

    T.H. Olfactory sensitivity during the menstrual

    cycle. Sensory Processes 2(2): 90-98 (1978).

    McCance, R.A., Luff, M.C. Widdowson, E.E. Physical

    and emotional periodicity in women. J. Hyg.

    (Can .)

    37: 571-611 (1937).

    Mitchell, G.D. Paternalistic behavior in primates. Psy-

    cho/. Bulletin 71(6): 399-417 (1969).

    Moos. R.H., Kopell, B.S. Melges, R.R. Yalom, I.D.

    Lunde,

    D.T. Clayton,

    R.B. Hamburg,

    D.A. Fluctuations in symptoms and moods during

    the menstrual cycle. J. Psychosom. Res. 13: 37-44

    (1969).

    Pfeiffer, J.E. The Emergence of Man. New York:

    Harper and Row, 1972.

    Preti, G., Huggins, G.R. 1975. Cyclical changes ir

    volatile acidic metabolites of human vaginal secre-

    tions and their relation to ovulation. J. Chem. Ecol.

    361-376 (1975).

    Schneider, R., Costiloe, P. Howard, R. Wolf,

    S. Olfactory perception thresholds in hypogonadal

    women: Changes accompanying the administration

    of androgen and estrogen. Journal of Clinical En-

    docrinology and Metabolism 18: 379-390 (1958).

    Spitz, C.J., Gold, A.R. Adams, D.B. Cognitive and

    hormonal factors affecting coital frequency. Arch.

    Sex. Eehav. 4: 249-263 (1975).

    Symons, D.

    The Evolurion of Human Sexuality. New

    York: Oxford Univ. Press, 1979.

    Trivers, R.L. Parental investment and sexual selec-

    tion. In Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man

    1871-1971, B. Campbell (Ed.). Chicago: Aldine,

    1972.

    Washburn, S.L., Lancaster, C.S. The evolution of

    hunting. In Human Evolution: Readings in Physical

    Anthropology, N. Kom and F.W. Thompson (Eds.).

    New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1967.