Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory,...

17
Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 brill.com/beh Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different levels of threat by the recluse spider Júlio M.G. Segovia a,b , Kleber Del-Claro a and Rodrigo Hirata Willemart b,c,d,a Instituto de Biologia, Bloco 2D 25, Campus Umuarama — Uberlândia — MG — CEP 38400-902, Brazil b Laboratório de Ecologia Sensorial e Comportamento de Artrópodes (LESCA), Escola de Artes Ciências e Humanidades, Universidade de São Paulo, Rua Arlindo Béttio, 1000 — Ermelino Matarazzo — 03828-000, São Paulo, SP, Brazil c Programa de Pós Graduação em Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil d Programa de Pós Graduação em Ecologia e Evolução, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil * Corresponding author’s e-mail address: [email protected] Accepted 9 December 2014; published online 13 January 2015 Abstract The threat sensitive hypothesis predicts that animals modulate the defensive behaviour with the level of threat. Therefore, responses to predator cues may differ from responses to the actual predator in close range. Also, in high threat situations, prey would be expected to use their most dangerous defences. The recluse spider Loxosceles gaucho (Araneae, Sicariidae) is known to prey upon well defended harvestmen such as the laniatorid Mischonyx cuspidatus (Opiliones, Gonyleptidae), which has been reported to use tanathosis, chemical defences, pinching with sharp apophyses on legs, chelicerae and pedipalps. Because of harvestmen’s dependence on chemical stimuli, we tested if M. cuspidatus would change its locomotory behaviour in the presence of chemicals of the recluse spider (low threat situation: spider vs blank vs chemical control; one at a time). Subsequently, we tested harvestmen behaviour in the presence of the spider in close range, a high-threat situation. Finally, we looked at the survival rate of spiders after being pierced by sharp apophyses that M. cuspidatus have on legs IV. The harvestmen only showed defensive behaviours in the high threat situation. Surprisingly, their mostly known defensive behaviours (chemical defence, tanathosis, pinching with chelicerae and pedipalps) were not seen even in the high threat situation. This is the first evidence that these behaviours are not used against a natural predator that has an almost 80% predation success when attacking harvestmen. Pinching with the sharp legs IV apophyses may perforate but do not kill the spiders. We highlight the importance © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2015 DOI 10.1163/1568539X-00003252

Transcript of Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory,...

Page 1: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 brill.com/beh

Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against differentlevels of threat by the recluse spider

Júlio M.G. Segovia a,b, Kleber Del-Claro a and

Rodrigo Hirata Willemart b,c,d,∗

a Instituto de Biologia, Bloco 2D 25, Campus Umuarama — Uberlândia — MG —CEP 38400-902, Brazil

b Laboratório de Ecologia Sensorial e Comportamento de Artrópodes (LESCA), Escola deArtes Ciências e Humanidades, Universidade de São Paulo, Rua Arlindo Béttio, 1000 —

Ermelino Matarazzo — 03828-000, São Paulo, SP, Brazilc Programa de Pós Graduação em Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências,

Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazild Programa de Pós Graduação em Ecologia e Evolução,Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

*Corresponding author’s e-mail address: [email protected]

Accepted 9 December 2014; published online 13 January 2015

AbstractThe threat sensitive hypothesis predicts that animals modulate the defensive behaviour with thelevel of threat. Therefore, responses to predator cues may differ from responses to the actualpredator in close range. Also, in high threat situations, prey would be expected to use their mostdangerous defences. The recluse spider Loxosceles gaucho (Araneae, Sicariidae) is known toprey upon well defended harvestmen such as the laniatorid Mischonyx cuspidatus (Opiliones,Gonyleptidae), which has been reported to use tanathosis, chemical defences, pinching with sharpapophyses on legs, chelicerae and pedipalps. Because of harvestmen’s dependence on chemicalstimuli, we tested if M. cuspidatus would change its locomotory behaviour in the presence ofchemicals of the recluse spider (low threat situation: spider vs blank vs chemical control; oneat a time). Subsequently, we tested harvestmen behaviour in the presence of the spider in closerange, a high-threat situation. Finally, we looked at the survival rate of spiders after being piercedby sharp apophyses that M. cuspidatus have on legs IV. The harvestmen only showed defensivebehaviours in the high threat situation. Surprisingly, their mostly known defensive behaviours(chemical defence, tanathosis, pinching with chelicerae and pedipalps) were not seen even in thehigh threat situation. This is the first evidence that these behaviours are not used against a naturalpredator that has an almost 80% predation success when attacking harvestmen. Pinching with thesharp legs IV apophyses may perforate but do not kill the spiders. We highlight the importance

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2015 DOI 10.1163/1568539X-00003252

Page 2: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

758 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

of the traditional descriptive approach with natural predators to understand the specificities ofdefensive behaviours against different types of predator.

KeywordsArachnida, Laniatores, defensive behaviour, deimatic, chemoreception.

1. Introduction

According to the threat sensitive hypothesis, modulating the defensive be-haviour with the level of threat is evolutionarily advantageous (Helfman,1989), and previous studies have indeed shown that many prey react differ-ently depending on the threat imposed: these reactions can be physiologicalresponses of stress (Monclús et al., 2009), increased vigilance time (Mathothet al., 2009), thanatosis (Gyssels & Stocks, 2005) and distinct behaviours ac-cording to the distance of the predator (Kleindorfer et al., 2005). Prey canexhibit a threat sensitive behaviour related to the size of the predator, forexample by avoiding only larger individuals (Chivers et al., 2001). In mites,latency to lay the first egg and the total number of eggs laid can also be af-fected by the level of threat by the presence of predator cues as well as to thepredator species (Ferrari & Schausberger, 2013). Finally, some monkeys canmodulate their behaviour according to the threat imposed by human presence(Papworth et al., 2013).

When animals face a low threat like chemical cues of predators, they canavoid the site, increase locomotion, decrease movements and flee (Kats &Dill, 1998 for a review). After the predator gets closer or grasp the prey,defensive behaviours such as retaliation come into play (Edmunds, 1974).Among the weapons used to retaliate predators, some seem to have evolvedfrom mechanisms of prey capture, others from functions related to intra-specific interactions and others are supposed to have evolved only for defen-sive reasons (Edmunds, 1974).

Harvestmen, arachnids belonging to the order Opiliones, have severalanti-predator behaviours. When facing a low threat situation such as thepresence of predator chemical cues, Eumesossoma roweri (Eupnoi: Sclero-somatidae) alters its locomotory pattern (Chelini et al., 2009). A presumablyhigh threat situation such as a human attempt to hold it causes harvestmen torelease defensive secretions produced by glands that open dorso-ventrally, toautotomise locomotory appendages, to shake the body, to flee, and to pinch

Page 3: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 759

the potential predator with apophyses on the legs, the so called ‘nipping be-haviour’ (the harvestman flexes legs IV so that the apophyses of the femurof the fourth pair of legs pinch against the body of the attacker) (Gnaspini &Hara, 2007).

Despite the great variety of defences that harvestmen possess, many ani-mals prey upon individuals of this group. The list includes birds, frogs, opos-sums, hemipterans and spiders (Gnaspini & Hara, 2007). Among neotropi-cal spiders, Ctenus fasciatus (Araneae, Ctenidae) feeds on harvestmen ofthe subfamily Goniosomatinae (Opiliones, Gonyleptidae) (Gnaspini, 1996;Machado et al., 2000) but there are no detailed studies on this prey–predatorsystem. The only pairs of spiders–harvestmen that have been studied in de-tails were spiders that do not usually eat harvestmen. These were carried outexactly with the purpose of understanding the proximal causes of rejection(Eisner et al., 2004; Souza & Willemart, 2011; Carvalho et al., 2012; Dias &Willemart, 2013; see also Machado et al., 2005). It has been shown that thehard integument is a highly important defence in these harvestmen belongingto the suborder Laniatores, and overcoming or avoiding it somehow is a pre-requisite to feed on these animals. We have shown that the delicate reclusespider Loxosceles gaucho (Gertsch, 1967) (Sicariidae) bites such prey at thefew vulnerable spots of the integument and is a very efficient predator ofsuch harvestmen (Segovia et al., in press). It seemed therefore to be a goodsystem to understand how prey react to different levels of threat by predators.In addition, we expected that most known defensive behaviours in laniatoridharvestmen would be revealed in interactions with such efficient predators.We therefore conducted a detailed behavioural analysis of the interaction be-tween the recluse spider and the syntopic harvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus(Roewer, 1913) (Gonyleptidae). We first tested whether these harvestmenchanged their locomotory behavioural pattern when exposed to chemicalcues of dangerous predators (a situation of low threat). Then we looked atthe defensive behaviours of the harvestmen when the spider is approaching,after spider’s touches and after spider’s bites (all situations considered to beof high threat based on data by Segovia et al., in press). Since preliminarydata showed that the sharp spines of legs IV in M. cuspidatus may perforatethe abdomen of a spider and because prey are expected to use their most dan-gerous weapons in situations of high threat, we also tested the hypothesis thatsuch sharp spines are lethal when the abdomen of the spider is perforated.

Page 4: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

760 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

2. Material and methods

2.1. Species studied

The spider Loxosceles gaucho is a predator that includes harvestmen in itsdiet because it is able to find and bite the weak points of the thick integumentof laniatorid harvestmen (Segovia et al., in press). This spider typically buildsa web sheet in which it is usually found, but it can also leave the web andcapture prey out of it (Fischer et al., 2006). In laboratory experiments ofpredation under different substrate conditions and in the presence or absenceof their web sheets, the success rate of prey capture was 77.3% with pooleddata, combining substrates and presence/absence of web (N = 66) (Segoviaet al., in press).

The harvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus has defensive behaviours that in-clude the release of defensive secretions by the ozopores (Hara et al., 2005),pinching with sharp apophyses on legs IV (Figure 1), thanatosis by flexingthe legs and keeping them close to the body (Pereira et al., 2004) and vibrat-ing legs II, a putative defensive behaviour recently described (ILT or intense

Figure 1. A harvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus (Arachnida, Opiliones) male, dorsal view.Black arrows show the sharp apophyses of the femur of the fourth pair of the legs. This figureis published in colour in the online edition of this journal, which can be accessed via http://booksandjournals.brillonline.com/content/journals/1568539x.

Page 5: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 761

leg tapping, see Dias et al., 2014). In the field, dead carcasses of M. cuspida-tus have been found on the webs of Loxosceles and theridiid spiders (Mestre& Pinto da Rocha, 2004, pers. obs.).

2.2. Collection and maintenance

Loxosceles gaucho was collected in a terrain located in the city of Mairiporã-SP, within a pile of bricks (23°19′S, 46°35′W). They were collected between24 November 2012 and 26 February 2013. Mischonyx cuspidatus was alsofound in that same area, but for this study they were collected at ParqueEcológico do Tietê — São Paulo (23°25′S, 46°28′W) between 20 December2012 and 8 May 2013. Both species co-occur in several locations (e.g., Dias& Willemart, 2013).

In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height) with soil on the bottom. Water was providedwith a wet cotton ball. We fed the spiders with larvae of the tenebrionidbeetle Zophobas sp. and nymphs of crickets (Gryllus sp.) according to theschedule of the experiments, the starvation period for spiders before the ex-periments varying between 5 and 12 days. Harvestmen were fed once a weekwith moistened dog food.

During the experiments, the animals were maintained in a room with aweak red light (15 W) on for 24 h per day. The natural daylight cycle wasmaintained and temperature was ambient. All experiments were carried outfrom 8:30 pm to 5 am.

2.3. Behaviour of the harvestmen in a low threat situation

This experiment was conducted to test the hypothesis that harvestmen reactto chemicals left on the substrate by dangerous predators. We predicted thatthe harvestmen would change their locomotory behaviour in the presenceof the spider chemicals (see Chelini et al., 2009). To test it, the harvestmen(N = 20) were exposed to filter paper in the following conditions, with onesingle stimulus per trial: (1) chemicals of spider; (2) a chemical control (matetea) and (3) a blank control. Mate tea is an adequate chemical control becauseharvestmen do not innately prefer to stay on it or avoid it, but they are ableto detect it (Santos et al., 2014). Each harvestman was tested three times ina design of repeated measures with an interval of 24 h between the tests foreach individual. Individuals in all treatments were tested in distinct hours ofthe night.

Page 6: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

762 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

To impregnate the filter paper with the spider cues, we maintained oneadult female of L. gaucho (5–10 days after it received its last meal) for 24 h ina container (8 cm diameter × 10 cm height) with filter paper on the bottom.We removed the spider immediately before the test. We then removed silkdeposited on the bottom with forceps, to prevent harvestmen getting stuckwithin it. In the chemical control group, we covered the filter paper with athin layer of mate tea and maintained it there for 24 h, removing it rightbefore the trials (no water was added, we only used powder). In the blankcontrol, the filter paper was not impregnated. In each group the containerwas closed while impregnating (the 24 h period before the test).

Tests were run in the same container where we impregnated the filter paperwith one of the chemicals (or control with no cues). We placed the filter paperon the bottom and a neutral central disc of filter paper (4 cm diameter) in thecentre. We first acclimated the harvestman in a vial (4 cm diameter × 7.5 cmheight) placed in the centre of the arena (in the neutral disk). After two min,the harvestman was released and we recorded it for 20 min with a SonyHDXR550V Handycam in nightshot mode. The harvestmen were submittedto each of the three treatments in a systematic alternated order: the sequenceof treatments was such that no sequence was more common than others andthere was no bias concerning the hour of the day and the treatment.

We compared three behavioural categories between the treatments: thetime harvestmen spent: 1, stationary (motionless); 2, stand waving (sta-tionary but leg waving); and 3, walking (displacing around the arena) (cf.,Chelini et al., 2009). We used a Friedman test (because the data did not fol-low a normal distribution), followed by a post-hoc Student–Neuman–Keulstest.

2.4. Behaviour of the harvestmen in a high threat situation

To describe the behaviour of the harvestmen in a high threat situation, wereanalysed videos used in an experiment for another project, which wasconducted to test, from the spider point of view, the role of silk on predation(Segovia et al., in press). Spiders in both the “no silk” group (N = 19,17 females and 2 males) and the “silk group” (N = 19, 13 females and6 males) were offered one harvestman each. We used 10 females and 9male harvestmen per group (38 harvestmen used in total). Twenty five to 29days before the trials (following Souza & Willemart, 2011), we offered onecricket nymph (Gryllus sp., ∼=1.5 cm) and one beetle larvae (Zophobas sp.)

Page 7: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 763

simultaneously to the spiders, to standardize hunger. Only spiders that ate atleast one prey were tested. Prey uneaten after 48 h were removed. The terrariaof the experiments (18 cm diameter × 6 cm height) were the same used tomaintain the spiders. During the tests we covered the terraria with a sheet ofglass to prevent the spiders from fleeing. Spiders in the treatment group weresimply removed and put back in their terrarium. For spiders in the controlgroup, we took them out of the terrarium, we removed all the soil, cleanedthe whole terrarium with a dry paper towel to remove remnants of silk andthen put new soil in it. This was done because removing silk was not possiblewithout removing the soil. We then reintroduced the spider. This proceduredid not affect the behaviour of the spider, as shown elsewhere (Segovia etal., in press). In both groups, we allowed the spider to walk freely in theterrarium for 15 min before we introduced a harvestman as far as possible ofthe spider.

We recorded the experiments using a Sony HDXR550V Handycam innightshot mode. We recorded for 40 min after the first bite. Each spider wastested only once. We collected the data between 21 February and 7 March2013, between 9 pm and 5 am. We used both the videos with the presenceand absence of silk because the predation success between the two treatmentswas the same (Segovia et al., in press). We only analysed videos in whichthe spider bit the harvestman. The behavioural analysis started about 5 sbefore the spider touched the harvestman and finished 2 min after the firstbite by the spider. We looked at the behavioural categories displayed by theharvestmen. Preliminary analyses of ten videos (5 silk group and 5 no-silkgroup) showed that increasing this time did not result in more behaviouralcategories observed.

2.5. Potential lethality of a harvestman defence under high threat

Nipping behaviour has been observed in interactions between M. cuspida-tus and the spiders Ctenus ornatus (Keyserling, 1891) (Ctenidae) and L.gaucho (Dias & Willemart, 2013; this paper). This behaviour is potentiallydangerous for a predator since M. cuspidatus has a sharp apophysis on theretrolateral portion of the femur of both legs IV. When they rapidly flextheir legs at the coxa-trochanter articulation (= ‘nipping’), the two apophy-ses pinch what is in between the legs. By experimentally holding the spiderby the prosoma and allowing a male M. cuspidatus to naturally pinch theabdomen of the spider with these apophyses, we found that the harvestman

Page 8: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

764 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

Figure 2. Opisthosoma of the recluse spider Loxosceles gaucho female injured by the retro-lateral apophysis of the femur of the harvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus male after a nipping.A droplet of haemolymph is shown by the arrow. Scale bar = 500 μm. This figure ispublished in colour in the online edition of this journal, which can be accessed via http://booksandjournals.brillonline.com/content/journals/1568539x.

is capable of perforating the abdomen of L. gaucho, which loses hemolymph(Figure 2). Because losing haemolymph seem to potentially cause death inanother harvestman (e.g., Willemart, 2002), we tested the hypothesis that aperforation caused by these apophyses negatively affects the survivorship ofthe spiders L. gaucho. We predicted that perforated spiders would have alower survival rate than the spiders that were not perforated.

We used two groups in this experiment. In the treatment group (N = 10),we anesthetised the spiders with CO2 by having them in a closed vial witha small aperture on the top in which the gas was injected for 1.5 min. Wethen held the spider within the femora of a male adult harvestman so thatwe could manually flex its legs, pinch and perforate the spider’s abdomen.We used a different harvestman for each spider and the same person heldthe harvestmen in every test. In the control group (N = 10), we used thesame procedures, but we pinched the abdomen with a cushioned forceps,

Page 9: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 765

again always by the same person. The experiment was carried out in May2013 between 8 pm and 3 pm. The harvestmen were then maintained inplastic containers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height) with soil on the bottom. Waterwas provided with a wet cotton ball. In the 5 subsequent days, we verifiedthe survivorship of the spiders between 10 and 11 pm and after 40 days wemonitored again. We compared the survival rate at the end of the experimentusing a χ2 test.

3. Results

3.1. Behaviour of the harvestman in a low threat situation

There were no differences when comparing the controls (blank and mate tea)and the treatment group (spider chemicals) in the time that the harvestmanspent: stationary (p = 0.071), stand waving (p = 0.170) or walking (p =0.744). The animals spent more time stand waving in all groups (p < 0.05):mate tea, blank and spider chemicals (Table 1). No defensive behaviour wasobserved.

3.2. Behaviour of the harvestman in a high threat situation

We found behaviours that can be considered potentially defensive in M.cuspidatus in different phases, as detailed below (see Table 2 for descrip-tions). The results are divided in ‘no-silk group’ (N = 15) and ‘silk group’(N = 17). Sample sizes provided below are the number of animals that haveshown each behaviour.

Table 1.Time spent by the harvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus in three behavioural categories, in threedifferent treatments: filter paper without stimuli; filter paper with mate tea (chemical control)or filter paper with predator chemicals.

Behavioural category/treatment Time spent (s) (median; MIN–MAX)

Blank Chemical control Spider chemicals

Stationary 49; 0–1087 0; 0–424 1; 0–1200Stand waving 903; 0–1200 1018; 361–1200 1024; 0–1200Walking 23; 0–452 78; 0–714 51; 0–399

The harvestmen were recorded for 1200 s. Each treatment was tested separately. MIN,minimum; MAX, maximum.

Page 10: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

766 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

Table 2.Behavioural repertory of the harvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus when interacting with therecluse spider Loxosceles gaucho, based on 32 predatory events.

Behavioural category Description

Intense leg tapping (ILT) Intense dorso-ventral movements of legs IIMotionless Not moving the body or the locomotory appendagesMove away from the spider The harvestman walks or try to walk away from the spiderMove toward the spider The harvestman walks or tries to walk toward the spider, often

when being held by the spiderMove pedipalps The harvestman displays dorso-ventral movements with the

pedipalpsNipping Pinching with legs IV that bear sharp apophysesPull the leg The harvestman quickly moves its legs dorso-ventrally,

probably trying to get rid of silk threadsSelf-grooming The harvestman passes legs and or pedipalps between the

chelicerae

Phase 1, pre-contact: In the no-silk group, we did not find behavioursconsidered as defensive. In the silk group, we have observed the behaviour‘pull the leg’ (N = 6).

Phase 2, post-contact, before biting: In the no-silk group, we have ob-served ‘move away from the spider’ (N = 3); ‘motionless’ (N = 2); ‘ILT’(N = 1) and ‘move toward the spider’ (N = 1). In the silk group we haveobserved ‘motionless’ (N = 4); ‘ILT’ (N = 1); ‘move away from the spider’(N = 1) and ‘move pedipalps’ (N = 1).

Phase 3, post-contact, after biting: In the no-silk group we have observed‘move toward the spider’ (N = 13); ‘move away from the spider’ (N = 8);‘motionless’ (N = 2); ‘move pedipalps’ (N = 1); ‘ILT’ (N = 1) and ‘nip-ping’ (N = 1). In the silk group, we have observed ‘pull the leg’ (N = 9);‘motionless’ (N = 2); ‘move away from the spider’ (N = 2) and ‘nipping’(N = 1). We did not observe the release of the defensive secretions, thanato-sis, and pinching with pedipalps and chelicerae in any of the phases analysed.

3.3. Potential lethality of a harvestman defence under high threat

One individual died in the control group in the first day and one died in thetreatment group in the second day. All the others stayed alive until the end ofthe 5 days in both treatments. There were no differences in the survival rateafter 40 days (χ2 = 0.00, df = 1, p = 1.00).

Page 11: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 767

4. Discussion

We observed defensive behaviours in M. cuspidatus in a high threat situationbut not in a low threat situation, in which we also did not detect differences inthe time the harvestmen spent stand waving, walking or stationary betweentreatments. Perforation of the spider’s body by the harvestman did not affectits survival.

Because no defensive behaviour at all could be noticed in the presence ofpredator chemicals, this is partially in accordance with previous studies thatshow prey changing its behaviour according to the level of threat (Sih et al.,1986; Bothan et al., 2008; Walzer & Schausberger, 2011). An example inarachnids is the spider Latrodectus hesperus (Theridiidae) that can modulateits defensive bites, doing it more frequently in a high threat situation than inmedium and low threat (Nelsen et al., 2014). But the lack of reaction of M.cuspidatus to chemical cues of L. gaucho contrasts with the results obtainedfor spiders (e.g., Persons et al., 2001, 2002; Wilder & Rypstra, 2004) andfor the harvestman Eumesosoma roeweri (Chelini et al., 2009) that exhibiteddefensive behaviours in the presence of chemicals of the spider Schizocosaocreata (Lycosidae) (Chelini et al., 2009). A possible explanation for thislack of defensive behaviour in a low threat situation is that the harvestmendid not detect the spider chemicals, but that seems unlikely since laniatoridshave chemoreceptors in the first and second pair of legs and are capable ofdetecting chemicals at close range or contact (Willemart & Chelini, 2007;Willemart et al., 2009; Costa & Willemart, 2013). An alternative hypothesisis that harvestmen do not avoid the chemicals because they cannot determinethe spider species only by their chemicals. Indeed, most syntopic spiders arenot efficient predators of M. cuspidatus, and unnecessary predator avoidancemay result in less weight gain (Chelini et al., 2009). Moreover, the hard in-tegument plays a great role in defence in species of the suborder Laniatores(Souza & Willemart, 2011; Carvalho et al., 2012; Dias & Willemart, 2014;Segovia et al., in press). The soft integument in Eupnoi could partially ex-plain why E. roeweri show defensive behaviours in the presence of predatorchemicals only (Chelini et al., 2009), but M. cuspidatus (with a hard pro-tective integument) do not. In any case, it should be considered that preyperform defensive behaviours more often towards specialists than towardsgeneralist predators (Crawford et al., 2012), such as Loxosceles spiders (Fis-cher et al., 2006).

Page 12: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

768 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

In the presence of the spider, in the pre-contact phase, we did not detectany defensive behaviour in the ‘no-silk group’. Only in the silk group weobserved the behaviour ‘pull the leg’, suggesting it is related to attempts ofgetting rid of the silk. Moving away from the spider is probably an attemptto escape, one of the most common defensive behaviours among animals(Edmunds, 1974). We also observed ILT (intense leg tapping), a behaviourfirst described in male–male fights (Willemart et al., 2009) and then in adefensive context (Dias et al., 2014). In our study, in one case L. gauchostopped the attack temporarily after this behaviour and in the other case thespider was touching the leg of M. cuspidatus and did not change its behaviourafter ILT.

In the phase 3 (post-contact after biting), the most common behaviourswe have observed were ‘move toward the spider’ and ‘move away fromthe spider’. We have also observed ‘motionless’, which is expected to beimportant against spiders since these animals typically rely also on vibratorycues to detect prey (Barth, 2002). But the recluse spiders attacked in all casesafter the harvestmen displayed this behaviour, which is in accordance withour data that vibratory cues may not be necessary for L. gaucho at least forcapturing harvestmen without silk (Segovia et al., in press). Further studiesshould be conducted to test which sensory modality is mainly used by thesespiders to detect their prey. Visual stimuli are known to be used for preycapture by spiders not known for their visual acuity such as ctenids (Pirhoferet al., 2007; Fenk et al., 2010). Air displacements detected by trichobothriaare another possibility (Barth, 2002).

Nipping behaviour has long being cited as an anti-predator behaviour (seereferences in Gnaspini & Hara, 2007), but only recently it has been observedagainst a natural predator (Dias & Willemart, 2013). Now we have shownthat it has the potential to pierce the body of a predator so it may not only bedeimatic, since losing haemolymph can be costly. At least in males of somecrickets, the loss of haemolymph can decrease reproductive success (Sakaluket al., 2004). Nonetheless, we do not know if the amount of haemolymph lostby the spiders in our study is enough to affect reproduction. Although ourresults provided no evidence of survival costs, in nature such injuries couldaffect spider behaviour or reduce the spider’s survivorship since animals aremore exposed to pathogens, and therefore open wounds can act as entry forbacteria (Loof et al., 2010).

Page 13: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 769

Some of the mostly known defences of laniatorids are the release of repug-natory chemicals and mechanical retaliation such as pinching with cheliceraeand pedipalps (Gnaspini & Hara, 2007). Mischonyx cuspidatus in particularis also known for displaying thanatosis for several minutes, bringing the legsclose to the body. This is the first evidence that none of these behavioursare observed against such a successful predator (almost 4 in each 5 attack-ing L. gaucho successfully feed on M. cuspidatus), and typically not againstother spiders too (Souza & Willemart, 2011; Carvalho et al., 2012; Dias &Willemart, 2013). Since most descriptions of chemical defences in harvest-men were made based on human manipulation (see references in Gnaspini& Hara, 2007), chemicals may be mainly used against vertebrate predatorsat least in some species that are more reluctant to release secretions, such asM. cuspidatus. Some anurans with toxic glands flex their legs when beingattacked and swallowed by predators, which often regurgitate them. Flexingthe legs may be a way to minimize the chances of injuries to the legs (seeToledo et al., 2010). The same could happen in M. cuspidatus, but it hasnever been tested against any natural vertebrate predator. At least the repug-natory chemicals of one laniatorid have been tested and proved to be effectiveagainst toads, but not opossums (Machado et al., 2005). A domestic dog hasalso been observed to be deterred by the secretions of Acanthogonyleptesfulvigranulatus (Gonyleptidae) (G.F. Pagoti, pers. commun.). After the dogsniffed on the harvestman, it released secretions on the dog’s nose. The dogsubsequently rubbed its nose against the substrate and did not chase the har-vestman. Since the dog only sniffed and did not bite, the possibility that CO2

from the predator influences the release of defensive secretions remains tobe tested.

We have shown that some of the most known defences of laniatorid har-vestmen such as releasing repugnatorial chemicals, pinching with cheliceraeand pedipalps and thanatosis are surprisingly not used against a very dan-gerous spider. Studies with arthropod defence and specifically chemical de-fences follow a tradition of manipulating with tweezers or by hand (e.g.,Eisner et al., 2005). Great advances were made, but several defensive be-haviours seen against tweezers or human hands are not used against somenatural predators. Such behaviours are probably specific to some predators.A clear methodological message of this and previous studies (Souza & Wille-mart, 2011; Carvalho et al., 2012; Cook et al., 2013; Dias & Willemart, 2013)

Page 14: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

770 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

is that ethologists have to be careful when designing experiments of prey–predator interactions. For example, knowing that a species releases defensivesecretions does not means it will release them against any predator. Testingthe effect of secretions on predators against which secretions are not used isunnatural, and this also applies to the huge number of arthropods and otheranimals that use chemical defences. The same applies to non-chemical de-fences. Finally, the traditional descriptive approach is crucial to unravel suchspecificities. As noted by Tinbergen (1963), “We must hope that the descrip-tive phase is not going to come to a premature ending”.

Acknowledgements

We thank the members of the Laboratory of Sensory Ecology and Behaviorof Arthropods, Gabriel Murayama, Guilherme Gainett, Guilherme Pagoti,Jessica Dias and Norton Silva-Santos, for revising an early draft of themanuscript. Marcelo Gonzaga and Michael Hrncir gave great suggestionsthat improved the manuscript, and the former also identified the spiders. Twoanonymous reviewers greatly helped commenting the manuscript. NathaliaFernandes, Norton Silva-Santos and Thaiany M. Costa helped collecting theanimals and Gabriel Murayama collecting preliminary data. Luiz Paulo An-drioli allowed the use of CO2. Ana Cristina Machado gave outstanding helpmaintaining the harvestmen in the laboratory. This study was funded byFAPESP to RHW, FAPEMIG to JMGS and CNPq to KDC.

References

Barth, F.G. (2002). A spider’s world: senses and behavior. — Springer, Dordrecht.Botham, M.S., Hayward, R.K., Morrell, L.J., Croft, D.P., Ward, J.R., Ramnarine, I. & Krause,

J. (2008). Risk-sensitive antipredator behavior in the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticu-lata. — Ecology 89: 3174-3185.

Carvalho, L.A., Souza, E.S. & Willemart, R.H. (2012). Analysis of the interaction betweenthe spitting spider Scytodes globula (Araneae: Scytodidae) and the harvestman Discocyr-tus invalidus (Opiliones: Gonyleptidae). — J. Arachnol. 40: 332-337.

Chelini, M.C., Willemart, R.H. & Hebets, E.A. (2009). Costs and benefits of freezing be-haviour in the harvestman Eumesosoma roeweri (Arachnida, Opiliones). — Behav. Proc.82: 153-159.

Chivers, D.P., Mirza, R.S., Bryer, P.J. & Kiesecker, J.M. (2001). Threat-sensitive predatoravoidance by slimy sculpins: understanding the importance of visual versus chemicalinformation. — Can. J. Zool. 79: 867-873.

Page 15: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 771

Cook, D.R., Smith, A.T., Proud, D.N., Víquez, C. & Townsend Jr., V.R. (2013). Defensiveresponses of Neotropical harvestmen (Arachnida, Opiliones) to generalist invertebratepredators. — Carib. J. Sci. 47: 334-343.

Costa, T.M. & Willemart, R.H. (2013). First experimental evidence that a harvestman (Arach-nida, Opiliones) detects odors of non-rotten dead prey by olfaction. — Zoologia 30:359-361.

Crawford, B.A., Hickman, C.R. & Luhring, T.M. (2012). Testing the threat sensitive hypoth-esis with predator familiarity and dietary specificity. — Ethology 118: 41-48.

Dias, B.C. & Willemart, R.H. (2013). The effectiveness of post-contact defenses in a preywith no pre-contact detection. — Zoology 116: 168-174.

Dias, B.C., Souza, E.S., Hara, M.R. & Willemart, R.H. (2014). Intense leg tapping by theharvestman Mischonyx cuspidatus (Gonyleptidae): an undescribed defensive behavior inOpiliones? — J. Arachnol. 42: 123-125.

Edmunds, M. (1974). Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. — Longman,Harlow.

Eisner, T., Rossini, C., González, A. & Eisner, M. (2004). Chemical defense of an opilionid(Acanthopachylus aculeatus). — J. Exp. Biol. 207: 1313-1321.

Eisner, T., Eisner, M. & Siegler, M. (2005). Secret weapons: defenses of insects, spiders,scorpions, and other many-legged creatures. — Harvard University Press, Cambridge,MA.

Fenk, L.M., Hoinkes, T. & Schmid, A. (2010). Vision as a third sensory modality to elicitattack in a nocturnal spider. — J. Comp. Physiol. 196: 957-961.

Ferrari, M.C.F. & Schausberger, P. (2013). From repulsion to attraction: species-and spatialcontext-dependent threat sensitive response of the spider mite Tetranychus urticae topredatory mite cues. — Naturwissenschaften 100: 541-549.

Fischer, M.L., Vasconcellos-Neto, J. & dos Santos Neto, L.G. (2006). The prey and predatorsof Loxosceles intermedia Mello-Leitao 1934 (Araneae, Sicariidae). — J. Arachnol. 34:485-488.

Gertsch, W.J. (1967). The spider genus Loxosceles in South America (Araneae, Scytodidae).— Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 136: 117-174.

Gnaspini, P. (1996). Population ecology of Goniosoma spelaeum a cavernicolous harvestmanfrom south-eastern Brazil (Arachnida: Opiliones: Gonyleptidae). — J. Zool. 239: 417-435.

Gnaspini, P. & Hara, M.R. (2007). Defense mechanisms. — In: Harvestmen: the biologyof Opiliones (Pinto-da-Rocha, R., Machado, G. & Giribet, G., eds). Harvard UniversityPress, Cambridge, MA, p. 374-399.

Gyssels, F.G. & Stoks, R. (2005). Threat-sensitive responses to predator attacks in a dam-selfly. — Ethology 111: 411-423.

Hara, M.R., Cavalheiro, A.J., Gnaspini, P. & Santos, D.Y. (2005). A comparative analysisof the chemical nature of defensive secretions of Gonyleptidae (Arachnida: Opiliones:Laniatores). — Bio. Syst. Ecol. 33: 1210-1225.

Helfman, G.S. (1989). Threat-sensitive predator avoidance in damselfish–trumpetfish inter-actions. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: 47-58.

Page 16: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

772 Defensive behaviour of a harvestman against a spider

Kats, L.B. & Dill, L.M. (1998). The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predationrisk by prey animals. — Ecoscience 5: 361-394.

Keyserling, E. (1891). Die Spinnen Amerikas. Brasilianische Spinnen 3. — Nürnberg.Kleindorfer, S., Fessl, B. & Hoi, H. (2005). Avian nest defence behaviour: assessment in

relation to predator distance and type, and nest height. — Anim. Behav. 69: 307-313.Loof, T.G., Schmidt, O., Herwald, H. & Theopold, U. (2010). Coagulation systems of inver-

tebrates and vertebrates and their roles in innate immunity: the same side of two coins?— J. Innat. Imm. 3: 34-40.

Machado, G., Raimundo, R.L.G. & Oliveira, P.S. (2000). Daily activity schedule, gregar-iousness, and defensive behaviour in the Neotropical harvestman Goniosoma longipes(Arachnida: Opiliones: Gonyleptidae). — J. Nat. Hist. 34: 587-596.

Machado, G., Carrera, P.C., Pomini, A.M. & Marsaioli, A.J. (2005). Chemical defense inharvestmen (Arachnida, Opiliones): do benzoquinone secretions deter invertebrate andvertebrate predators? — J. Chem. Ecol. 31: 2519-2539.

Mathot, K.J., van den Hout, P.J. & Piersma, T. (2009). Differential responses of red knotsCalidris canutus to perching and flying sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus models. — Anim.Behav. 77: 1179-1185.

Mestre, L.A.M. & Pinto-da-Rocha, R. (2004). Population dynamics of an isolated popula-tion of the harvestman Ilhaia cuspidata (Opiliones, Gonyleptidae), in Araucaria Forest(Curitiba, Paraná, Brazil). — J. Arachnol. 32: 208-220.

Monclús, R., Palomares, F., Tablado, Z., Martínez-Fontúrbel, A. & Palme, R. (2009). Testingthe threat-sensitive predator avoidance hypothesis: physiological responses and predatorpressure in wild rabbits. — Oecologia 158: 615-623.

Nelsen, D.R., Kelln, W. & Hayes, W.K. (2014). Poke but don’t pinch: risk assessment andvenom metering in the western black widow spider, Latrodectus hesperus. — Anim.Behav. 89: 107-114.

Papworth, S., Milner-Gulland, E.J. & Slocombe, K. (2013). Hunted woolly monkeys(Lagothrix poeppigii) show threat-sensitive responses to human presence. — PloS ONE8: e62000.

Pereira, W., Elpino-Campos, A., Del-Claro, K. & Machado, G. (2004). Behavioral repertoryof the neotropical harvestman Ilhaia cuspidata (Opiliones, Gonyleptidae). — J. Arachnol.32: 22-30.

Persons, M.H., Walker, S.E., Rypstra, A.L. & Marshall, S.D. (2001). Wolf spider predatoravoidance tactics and survival in the presence of diet-associated predator cues (Araneae,Lycosidae). — Anim. Behav. 61: 43-51.

Persons, M., Walker, S.E. & Rypstra, A.L. (2002). Fitness costs and benefits of antipredatorbehavior mediated by chemotactile cues in the wolf spider Pardosa milvina (Araneae:Lycosidae). — Behav. Ecol. 13: 386-392.

Pirhofer-Walzl, K., Warrant, E. & Barth, F.G. (2007). Adaptations for vision in dim light:impulse responses and bumps in nocturnal spider photoreceptor cells (Cupiennius saleiKeys). — J. Comp. Physiol. 193: 1081-1087.

Roewer, C.F. (1913). Die Familie der Gonyleptiden der Opiliones-Laniatores. — Arch.Naturgeschichte 79: 1-472.

Page 17: Defences of a Neotropical harvestman against different ...& Willemart, 2013). In the laboratory, they were both individually maintained in plastic con-tainers (12 × 8 × 4 cm height)

J.M.G. Segovia et al. / Behaviour 152 (2015) 757–773 773

Sakaluk, S.K., Campbell, M.T., Clark, A.P., Johnson, J.C. & Keorpes, P.A. (2004).Hemolymph loss during nuptial feeding constrains male mating success in sagebrushcrickets. — Behav. Ecol. 15: 845-849.

Santos, G.C., Hogan, J.A. & Willemart, R.H. (2013). Associative learning in a harvestman(Arachnida, Opiliones). — Behav. Proc. 100: 64-66.

Segovia, J.M.G., Del-Claro, K. & Willemart, R.H. (2015). Delicate fangs, smart killing: thepredation strategy of the recluse spider. — Anim. Behav., in press.

Sih, A. (1986). Antipredator responses and the perception of danger by mosquito larvae. —Ecology 67: 434-441.

Souza, E.D.S. & Willemart, R.H. (2011). Harvest-Ironman: heavy armature, and not its de-fensive secretions, protects a harvestman against a spider. — Anim. Behav. 81: 127-133.

Toledo, L.F., Sazima, I. & Haddad, C.F.B. (2010). Is it all death feigning? Case in anurans.— J. Nat. Hist. 44: 1979-1988.

Walzer, A. & Schausberger, P. (2011). Threat-sensitive anti-intraguild predation behaviour:maternal strategies to reduce offspring predation risk in mites. — Anim. Behav. 81: 177-184.

Wilder, S.M. & Rypstra, A.L. (2004). Chemical cues from an introduced predator (Mantodea,Mantidae) reduce the movement and foraging of a native wolf spider (Araneae, Lycosidae)in the laboratory. — Environ. Entomol. 33: 1032-1036.

Willemart, R.H. (2002). Cases of intra- and inter-specific food competition among Brazilianharvestmen, in captivity (Opiliones, Laniatores, Gonyleptidae). — Rev. Arachnol. 14: 49-58.

Willemart, R.H. & Chelini, M.C. (2007). Experimental demonstration of close-range olfac-tion and contact chemoreception in the Brazilian harvestman, Iporangaia pustulosa. —Entomol. Exp. Appl. 123: 73-79.

Willemart, R.H., Farine, J.P. & Gnaspini, P. (2009). Sensory biology of Phalangida harvest-men (Arachnida, Opiliones): a review, with new morphological data on 18 species. —Acta Zool. 90: 209-227.