La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

download La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

of 10

Transcript of La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    1/10

    Plasticity of motor cortex induced by coordination and training

    F. Tyc*, A. Boyadjian

    Laboratoire Plasticit et Physio-Pathologie de la Motricit, UMR 6196, CNRS, 31 Chemin J. Aiguier, 13402 Marseille Cedex 20, France

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:

    Accepted 24 May 2010

    Available online 17 June 2010

    Keywords:

    TMS

    Motor cortex

    Training

    Coordination

    Plasticity

    Mapping

    a b s t r a c t

    Objective: To study the modifications induced by training of a coordinated movement on the primary

    motor cortex (M1) maps of one proximal muscle and one distal muscle activated alone and during their

    co-contraction.Methods: Six healthy female sport students performed a 6-week training program during which they

    were trained in darts 34 times a week. At the end each subject had made more than 1200 throws. Trans-

    cranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was used to map the proximal medial deltoid (MD) and the distal

    brachio-radialis (BR) muscle representations on M1. Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) amplitude and

    excitability curves were used to test corticomotor excitability.

    Results: The cortical representation areas of each muscle separately increased after training. The cortical

    representation and the excitability curve of the BR muscle increased during co-activation with the MD.

    Combining co-contraction and training produced a further enlargement of the M1 representation of

    the BR muscle.

    Conclusions: The enlargement of the BR representation in M1 suggests the development of overlapping

    zones specifying functional synergies between distal and proximal muscles.

    Significance: Our findings support the idea that training of a coordinated movement involving several

    muscles and joints requires an activity-dependent coupling of cortical networks.

    2010 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights

    reserved.

    1. Introduction

    In healthy humans, theabilityto learn novel motor skillsthrough

    practice is accompanied by functional reorganization of the motor

    system including the primary motor cortex (M1) (Pascual-Leone

    et al., 1994; Karni et al., 1995; Nudo et al., 1996; Classen et al.,

    1998). Motor practiceinduces changes in cortical motorrepresenta-

    tion that can be measured with transcranial magnetic stimulation

    (TMS). The changes are principally an enlargement of the cortical

    representation of the muscle used in the task and a facilitation of

    the motor evoked potentials (MEPs), indicating an increase in corti-

    cal excitability. In short-term motor learning studies, cortical

    changes can be observed for a few minutes (Garry et al., 2004),

    whereas learning a piano sequence produces more pronounced

    changes (Pascual-Leone et al.,1994, 1995). Classen et al. (1998) used

    TMS to evoke isolated and directional thumb movements and

    showed that training modified the thumb cortical networks, which

    encoded movement kinetics. This suggests a short-term memory

    for movement that is the first step in skill acquisition ( Classen

    et al., 1998; Karni et al., 1995). Most of the cited studies were

    focused on distal hand muscles known to have larger cortical

    representations and explored the effects of short-term plasticity

    on their cortical representations.

    TMS has been used to map cortical representations of different

    muscles at rest (Wassermann et al., 1992) and during low level

    activity (Wilson et al., 1995). Motor cortex representations in hu-

    mans have shown discrete amplitude peaks included within a dif-

    fuse representation. Maps of different upper extremity muscles

    overlapped on the motor cortex in spite of a lateral shift (Penfield

    and Rasmussen, 1950). The threshold for the activation of proximal

    muscles by transcranial electrical stimulation (TES, Rothwell et al.,

    1987) or TMS (Wassermann et al., 1992) was higher than for distal

    muscles. A low level voluntary contraction permitted the observa-

    tion of MEPs at a lower rate of stimulation ( Wilson et al., 1995),

    especially for proximal muscles of the upper extremities that were

    difficult to map at rest (Levy et al., 1991; see Amassian et al., 1995).

    Since Penfield and Boldrey (1937) and Penfield (1950), the large

    representation of distal muscles has been generally associated with

    a fine level of control. Cortical maps revealed some size differences

    depending on handedness and the muscle location (proximal

    versus distal). Wassermann et al. (1992) showed that distal muscle

    representations were larger than proximal ones, and that they

    were larger for the dominant side, contrary to proximal muscles.

    A few studies have investigated the changes of proximal muscle

    representation induced by motor practice (see Tyc and Boyadjian,

    1388-2457/$36.00 2010 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.022

    * Corresponding author. Tel./fax: +33 3 20 74 85 66.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (F. Tyc).

    Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / c l i n p h

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.022mailto:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.022http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/13882457http://www.elsevier.com/locate/clinphhttp://www.elsevier.com/locate/clinphhttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/13882457http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.022mailto:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.022
  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    2/10

    2006). Liepert et al. (1999) have shown that short-term training of

    synchronous thumb and foot movements modified motor output

    maps due to interactions between hand and foot representation

    areas in M1. This result indicated a shift of the thumb motor output

    map towards the leg map. Changes in hand muscle corticomotor

    excitability in relation to static shoulder positions have been inter-

    preted as a proximaldistal synergism operating through reaching

    movements (Ginanneschi et al., 2005). More recently, comparisonof the effects of short-term training at proximal and distal upper

    limb joints, revealed a greater effect on the pathways controlling

    the index finger and a smaller effect on pathways controlling more

    proximal joints (Krutky and Perreault, 2007). These findings sug-

    gested a difference in the motor cortical contributions to short-

    term motor plasticity in the pathways controlling proximal and

    distal joints in the upper limb. Ackerley et al. (2007) used TMS to

    explore whether training in different coordination patterns affects

    the development of motor plasticity. A simple repetitive thumb

    movement made in synchrony, or in syncopation, with respect to

    an auditory metronome, shifted both synchronized and syncopated

    motor practice modified thumb movements in the trained direc-

    tion. These studies have raised questions about the role of the mo-

    tor cortex in coordinating multi-joint movements and the impact

    of training. To date, it has not been established whether long-term

    training of a complex coordinated movement affects the cortical

    representation of muscles during their co-activation.

    In a previous study, we examined the effect of highly-skilled

    behaviour on motor cortex representations of upper extremity

    muscles in volleyball players. We showed an expansion of proximal

    muscle representation in the contralateral M1 with an increase in

    overlap between proximal and distal muscle representations (Tyc

    et al., 2005). This overlap was suggested to play a role in muscle

    control during coordinated multi-joint movements. It was consis-

    tent with other findings that generated the hypothesis that M1

    could control the different limb segments as a whole rather than

    individually (Scott 2000). For example, shoulder, elbow and wrist

    muscle activation during pointing movements appeared to involve

    common motor cortical circuits (Devanne et al., 2002). As Schieber(2001) has suggested, an organization of the motor cortex with lar-

    ger representations of the proximal muscles overlapping those of

    the distal muscles might facilitate coordination and motor learning.

    The purpose of the present study was to use TMS to further ana-

    lyze the changes in cortical representations of a shoulder muscle

    and of a forearm muscle used to execute a complex coordinated

    movement. The key parameters in our protocol were the learning

    of a coordinated complex movement, the co-contraction of a prox-

    imal and a distal muscle, and long-term motor training. We chose

    the traditional game of darts, in which all joints in an upper limb

    are involved in a specific coordination pattern and timing of

    proximal and distal muscles (Temprado et al., 1997). We report

    the changes in the cortical representations of the proximal

    medial deltoid (MD) and the distal brachio-radialis (BR) musclesfollowing a six-week training period together with an enlargement

    of the scalp-evoked motor maps when the two muscles are

    co-contracted.

    Part of this work has been presented in abstract form (Tyc and

    Boyadjian, 2008).

    2. Methods

    2.1. Subjects

    Studies conforming to the standards set by the declaration of

    Helsinki were carried out on six healthy female sport students

    (mean age 21 0.26) who did not regularly participate in a sportinvolving specific arm activity. All subjects gave their free informed

    written consent for the study. All subjects were right-handed

    according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971).

    2.2. Training protocol

    The subjects participated in a six-week training program during

    which they trained at throwing darts 34 times a week. Each sub-

    ject received a complete dart game set including a dartboard andthree darts. The dartboard was used on the side showing 10 con-

    centric circles worth one point for the external circle and 10 points

    for the centre. The throwing distance was indicated by a black line

    on the floor set at 2.4 m from the dartboard. The subject had her

    right hip facing the target, with her right foot on the black line

    and threw the dart with her right arm. In this specific position

    MD and BR muscles are co-contracted as shown by EMG recordings

    (Fig. 1). During each training session the subject had to maintain

    the same position at the same distance from the dartboard and

    place darts as close as possible to the centre of the target in order

    to obtain a maximum number of points. Each session lasted 15

    20 min and consisted of 6080 throws. At the end of the six weeks,

    each subject had made more than 1200 throws. Before starting the

    six-week training program and at the end of the six-week training

    program, each subject performed a test with 15 throws. The mean

    point obtained per dart was calculated by summing the number of

    points of the 15 throws and then dividing by 15. Comparison of the

    two scores provided an evaluation of the effect of training.

    2.3. Electromyographic recordings (EMG)

    The skin was prepared for EMG recordings obtained from paired

    surface electrodes placed over the belly of the MD and BR muscles

    of the right arm (see Fig. 1). A large reference electrode was placed

    around the wrist. EMGs were recorded over a 250 ms time window

    including 50 ms prior to the stimulus. EMG signals were amplified

    (1000) with high pass filtering at 10 Hz and low pass filtering at

    1 kHz before being sampled at 2 kHz by an A/D converter and

    stored on a computer for off-line analysis.

    2.4. Transcranial magnetic stimulation

    A snugly fitting cap was positioned over the subjects head and

    a grid was drawn with stimulus sites spaced 1.5 cm from the ver-

    tex using the nasion-inion line and the inter-aural line as refer-

    ences. TMS was delivered using a figure-of-eight coil with

    external loop diameter of 9 cm connected to a MagStim 200 elec-

    tromagnetic stimulator (maximum output intensity 2.0 T, Mag-

    stim, Dyfed, UK). The coil was held tangentially to the skull

    positioned at 45 in relation to the nasion-inion line with the han-

    dle to the rear so that the induced monophasic current flow was in

    a posterior to anterior direction. This is the most favorable orienta-

    tion for activating the corticospinal tract transynaptically (Kanekoet al., 1996). The junction of the coil was placed over the site to be

    stimulated on the left hemisphere that elicited MEPs in the right

    contralateral target muscles.

    2.5. Experimental protocol

    Two sessions of identical measurements were carried out. The

    first one was carried out before the training period (control) and

    the second one at the end of the training period (six weeks later).

    To record EMG activity, subjects were comfortably seated and

    adopted a static position in order to produce constant low level

    activity in each muscle. EMG activity was displayed on line on a

    computer screen to allow the subject to maintain a constant and

    stable low EMG activity set at 5% of the maximal EMG activity.Two sets of 2 s recordings were made to measure EMG activity,

    154 F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    3/10

    the first during low level activity produced during mapping and

    the second during maximum voluntary contraction. The value of

    the rectified EMG during low activity was divided by the values ob-

    tained during maximal EMG activity to quantify the activity during

    mapping. The mean values of the rectified EMG during low level

    activity were stable at 5% of the maximal EMG activity in both ses-

    sions. For MD recordings, the subject placed their arm in a stableposition aiming at a target to throw a dart. For BR, recordings were

    obtained under two conditions. (1) Alone condition: the subject

    maintained the 5% low activity in the BR muscle in a static position

    with the forearm on an arm-rest. Particular care was taken to en-

    sure that only the BR muscle was contracted and no EMG activity

    could be recorded in the MD muscle as verified online at high mag-

    nification on the screen. Mapping only started when the MD mus-

    cle was silent. (2) Co-active condition: the subject maintained 5%

    low EMG activity in both muscles (BR and MD).

    2.6. Motor output mapping

    Prior to mapping, the hot spot and the active motor threshold

    (aMT) were determined for each muscle. The hot spot was the siteat which the largest MEP could be induced with suprathreshold

    single stimuli applied with TMS. The aMT was defined as the low-

    est stimulation intensity that induced a peak-to-peak MEP ampli-

    tude of more than 200lV in 5 out of 10 trials in the target

    muscle. The TMS intensity was then adjusted to 1.2 times the

    aMT of each muscle for mapping. Three TMS response maps were

    established on the left hemisphere: one for MD muscle and two

    for BR muscle (Alone and Co-active condition). Four stimuli were

    delivered at each grid site with intervals randomly ranging be-

    tween 3 and 5 s. The four recordings were averaged and stored

    on a computer for further off-line analysis.

    2.7. Excitability curves

    Excitability curves (Ridding and Rothwell, 1997) were obtained

    by stimulating the BR hot spot with ten increasing TMS intensities

    (from 1.2 to 1.65 aMT) under two conditions. Alone condition,

    with only the BR muscle contracted and Co-active condition with

    the two muscles (BR and MD) contracted. At each intensity, three

    stimuli were applied and MEPs stored for further analysis.

    2.8. Data analysis

    To determine the cortical representation of each muscle the

    four non-rectified EMG recordings were averaged for each stimula-

    tion site. The peak-to-peak amplitude of the MEPs was measured

    and plotted on medio-lateral and antero-posterior coordinates.

    For each subject and for each muscle, a contour plot was drawnwith its external borderline corresponding to the amplitude of

    the minimal MEP (2D representation). The area of the contour plot,

    i.e. the muscle map representation on the scalp in cm2 was then

    calculated.

    For 3D representation, the Lorentzian 3D function was used to

    fit data by the LevenbergMarquard nonlinear least-mean-square

    algorithm (Press et al., 1986; Tyc et al., 2005; Devanne et al.,

    2006). This algorithm determines the function parameters thatminimize the sum of the squared differences between the observed

    and the predicted values of the dependent variable using a gradient

    descent-based optimization procedure. Before fitting, the baseline

    was adjusted to the mean rectified EMG activity recorded during

    the 2 s low level activity. The Lorentzian equation relating the

    peak-to-peak amplitude of the MEP and the antero-posterior (y)

    and medio-lateral (x) coordinates was given by the following

    equation:

    ZZmax

    1 xx0a

    2 1 yy0

    b

    2

    Zmax is the estimated amplitude of the maximal MEP that is located

    at coordinates x0 and y0. The parameters a and b reflect the

    straightness of the peak in the medio-lateral and antero-posterior

    directions, respectively. It has been shown that the estimated coor-

    dinates of the theoretical hot spot (x0 and y0) of each muscles rep-

    resentation were within the area from which MEPs were evoked

    and very close to the site at which the largest response was ob-

    tained experimentally (Devanne et al. 2006).

    For excitability curve fitting, we used the Boltzmann sigmoidal

    function where MEPmax represents the plateau level, S50 the stim-

    ulus intensity required to obtain the 50% plateau value, and k re-

    flects the slope of the curve:

    MEP MEPmax

    1 ess50=k

    For statistics a two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements

    was calculated. Muscle representation variables (2D and 3D) and

    the excitability curves were compared between sessions for both

    muscles and between both conditions for BR muscle (Alone and

    Co-active) using a paired t-test as appropriate. To test the effect

    of independent variables on excitability curves, we used a t-test

    based on the standard error of estimate associated with each

    best-fit parameter in each subject. The level of significance was

    set at 5%.

    3. Results

    The six-week training period induced an increase in the

    performance in the throwing test for each subject. Before training,the 15 throws test resulted in a score between 47 and 89 points

    Fig. 1. Photo showing the position of the throwing arm and the place of the electrodes on MD and BR muscles. Right, rectified EMG obtained during the three phases of

    throwing. Note the co-activation of MD and BR muscles during throwing.

    F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162 155

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    4/10

    with a mean of 70 points for the six subjects (4.7 points per dart).

    The score after training was 77 to 112 points with a mean of 94

    points (6.3 points per dart).

    The aMT were stable and no statistical difference was observed

    throughout the training session for each subject and for both mus-

    cles. The aMT of the BR muscle expressed as a percentage of the

    maximal stimulator output were 34.8 1.72% (mean SEM) before

    the training program and 34 2.08% after training. For the MD

    muscle the aMT were 37.7 2.69% and 37 2.21% before and after

    the training program.

    By stimulating M1 and recording the MEPs from the target mus-

    cles, an estimate of the representation of each muscle was obtained

    on the scalp. Fig. 2 is an example of the MEPs obtained for the MD

    and BR muscle representations before and after six-weeks training

    in one subject. The location of MEP traces on the grid corresponds

    to the scalp position from which they were evoked. The same

    Fig. 2. Mean of 4 MEPs produced by TMS in MD and BR muscles at each scalp position in one subject before and after the 6 weeks training session. The location of the MEP

    traces on the grid corresponds to the scalp sites from where they were evoked. V, vertex position.

    156 F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    5/10

    protocol was applied to each subject. The data obtained for the six

    subjects before and after training on MD muscle are provided in

    Table 1. The mean number of sites from which a MEP could be in-

    duced in the MD muscle increased from 13 to 17. The mean surface

    area of the MD muscle representation containing the sites in-

    creased significantly (p < 0.006) from 27 to 35 cm2 after training.

    This modification is illustrated by the contour plots obtained in

    one subject in Fig. 3A. After the training session the mean motor

    area for the MD muscle represented 129% of the mean motor area

    obtained before training.

    For the BR muscle cortical map area, the interaction between ses-

    sion (before and after training) and condition (Alone and Co-active)

    was tested with two way repeated ANOVA. No statistically signifi-

    cant interaction was found (F= 0.0546, P= 0.824). The BR muscle

    representation was determined before and after training, as was

    the representation of the MD muscle (Fig. 2). Training induced sim-

    ilar increasesin the number of MEPs andmap area forthe BRmuscle

    as was observed forthe MD musclerepresentation (Fig.3B).The size

    of the surface area of the cortical representation of the BR after

    training was significantly different from before training (p < 0.02).

    The results are given in Table 2 for the six subjects (BR alone).

    3.1. The effects of co-contraction

    Mapping was performed to evaluate the effect of co-contraction

    on the representation of the BR muscle (Fig. 2, BR Co-active). The

    surface area of the BR representation increased during co-contrac-

    tion as shown in Fig. 3C. Table 2 provides the results for the six

    subjects (BR Co-active). The mean number of cortical sites from

    which a MEP could be obtained on the BR muscle in the Co-active

    condition was 19 compared to 16 in the Alone condition. The mean

    BR muscle representation area was 39 cm2 in the Co-Active Condi-

    tion and was significantly larger (p < 0.02) than the 31 cm2 when

    the BR muscle was tested alone.

    3.2. Dual effects of co-contraction and training

    The effect of co-contraction of both muscles was measured on

    the BR muscle representation before and after the training session.

    Table 1

    Map areas and number of MEPs for MD muscle for six subjects before and after the

    6 weeks training. Means are shown SEM.

    MD map area (cm2) Number of MEP

    Before After % Before After %

    1 37.1 47 127 15 22 147

    2 21.2 34.9 165 12 16 133

    3 32.3 35.9 111 17 17 100

    4 22.4 26.4 118 11 14 127

    5 22.6 30.7 136 12 16 133

    6 27.4 32.1 117 13 17 131

    Mean 27 2.6 35 2.9 129 8.0 13 0.9 17 1.1 128 6.3

    Fig. 3. Cortical representations obtained in one subject for MD (A) and BR (B) muscles before (straight line) and after (dotted line) training. C and D, cortical representations ofBR muscle obtained in the Alone (arrows) and the Co-Active (dashed line) Conditions before training (C) and after training (D).

    F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162 157

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    6/10

    One example obtained in one subject is shown in Fig. 3C and D

    where an increase in the surface area of the BR muscle was ob-

    served. Data for the six subjects are given in Table 2, confirming

    the effect of co-contraction and the effect of training on the mean

    surface area of BR muscle representation. The mean number of cor-

    tical sites from which a MEP could be obtained on the BR in the Co-

    Active Condition was 19 before training against 22 after training.

    The 48 cm2 of the mean surface area of the BR muscle reached after

    training must be compared to the 39 cm2 measured before train-

    ing. The increase was significant (p < 0.02) and represented 122%

    of the map area before training.

    3.3. 3D representation of the MD and BR muscles

    To estimate the coordinates (x0, y0) of the theoretical hot spots,

    a 3D representation of the muscles was calculated in order to fol-

    low the modification of the location of the hot spots in the three

    experimental situations. Fig. 4A illustrates the modification ob-

    tained on 3D representations after training for both muscles. It

    indicates an increase in the cortical excitability associated with

    an increase of the cortical surface from which the muscle could

    be activated. The mean R2 values were between 0.71 and 0.84 for

    the six subjects. The distances between the theoretical hot spots

    of the MD and the BR (Table 3) were significantly different

    (p < 0.03) in the Alone Condition but not in the Co-Active Condition

    (p < 0.06). For one subject fig. 4B (left) shows that the co-contrac-

    tion induced a reduction of the inter-spot distance from 18 to

    7.5 mm before training. After training, the inter-spot distance

    was 4.1 mm for the BR alone while the location of the hot-spots

    nearly coincided (0.5 mm) in the Co-Active Condition. The histo-

    grams in Fig. 4B (right) show the differences in inter-spot distances

    between before and after training. In the Alone Condition a reduc-

    tion of the inter-spot distances was observed in five out of six sub-

    jects and in all subjects in the Co-Active Condition. The reduction

    emphasizes the dual effect of training and co-contraction.

    3.4. Excitability curves

    The excitability curves measured on the BR muscle showed an

    increased MEP area when the stimulation intensity was increased

    before and after the training session under the Alone and Co-Active

    Condition (Fig. 5, left). During co-activation, the plateau levels ofthe curves reached higher values in each subject and for each

    session compared to the levels reached when the BR muscle was

    activated alone. The histograms of Fig. 5 (right) confirm that the

    values of the plateau levels obtained from the excitability curves

    before and after training for the six subjects were systematically

    and significantly higher during co-contraction (142% and 159% be-

    fore and after training). The increase in cortical excitability re-

    sulted from the combined effects of training and co-contraction.

    4. Discussion

    The principal finding of the present study is that co-activation

    of the proximal MD muscle with the distal BR muscle enlarges

    the cortical representation of the BR muscle and that a six-weeks

    training period produces an enlargement of the cortical motor rep-resentations of the co-contracted muscle.

    4.1. Condition of recordings

    At rest, proximal muscles are difficult to map (Levy et al., 1991;

    Amassian et al., 1995). Stimulus intensitycan reach 80%of the max-

    imal intensity of the stimulator that is approximately twice the

    intensity used during low level activity (Tyc and Boyadjian, 2006).

    In contrast, when a muscle is slightly activated, the MEP threshold

    is decreased and TMS mapping is facilitated (Wilson et al., 1995).

    Our experimental protocol relies on these observations.

    4.2. Location of training-dependent plasticity

    The increase of MEP size induced by co-activation and training

    reflects a modification of the excitability of the cortico-spinal sys-

    tem as a whole without providing evidence that plasticity occurs at

    cortical levels. However, it can be argued that the observed effects

    on corticomotor excitability cannot exclusively operate at the

    spinal level even though the spinal neuronal circuitry has been

    shown to exhibit adaptive plasticity in experimental animals

    (Wolpaw and Carp, 1993) and in humans (Baylor and Benjuya,

    1989). Because TES produces a greater proportion of direct activa-

    tion of corticospinal neurons than TMS (Day et al., 1987a, b;

    Rothwell, 1997) and because training induces an increase in MEPs

    evoked by TMS but not by TES brain stem or cervical stimulation

    (Classen et al., 1998; Muellbacher et al., 2001; Perez et al., 2004;

    Garry et al., 2004), this form of plasticity is more likely to occurat the level of the cortical networks than at subcortical levels.

    Table 2

    Map areas and number of MEPs for BR muscles of six subjects before and after the 6 weeks training in the two conditions: Alone (only BR muscle contracted) and Co-Active (BR

    and MD muscles co-contracted). Means are shown SEM.

    BR Alone BR Co-Active % Co-Active/Alone

    Before After % Before After % Before After

    Map area (cm2)

    1 38.4 58.8 153 54.5 60.1 110 142 102

    2 31.7 39.3 124 34.5 42.6 123 109 109

    3 39.1 42.8 110 41.2 50.3 122 105 118

    4 28.6 40.3 141 39.9 54.5 137 140 135

    5 24.8 29.8 120 31.3 34.6 111 126 116

    6 25.3 32.6 129 34.1 44.9 132 135 138

    Mean 31 2.6 41 4.2 129 6.3 39 3.4 48 3.7 122 4.4 126 6.4 120 5.8

    Number of MEP

    1 17 25 147 22 26 118 129 104

    2 17 20 118 17 23 135 100 115

    3 19 20 105 20 21 105 105 105

    4 16 19 119 19 24 126 119 126

    5 13 15 115 15 18 120 115 120

    6 14 18 129 18 20 111 129 111

    Mean 16 0.9 20 1.3 122 5.8 19 1.0 22 1.2 119 4.4 116 4.9 113 3.5

    158 F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    7/10

    4.3. Reproducibility of cortical M1 maps

    Although there can be no doubt that the brain displays plastic-

    ity, stability of cortical networks is also needed, as discussed in a

    recent review by Wandell and Smirnakis (2009).

    That motor cortical map representations are reproducible insubjects over a long period of time is well documented. The

    TMS-mapped representations of two intrinsic hand muscles were

    found to be very reproducible when mapping was repeated after

    intervals of up to 181 days (Wilson et al., 1993). A method for

    mapping the motor cortex by TMS was tested statistically for its

    reproducibility and found to be reliable for mapping the human

    motor cortex (Mortifee et al., 1994). Maeda et al. (2002) investi-gated the reproducibility of mapping techniques on six normal

    Fig. 4. (A) Three-dimensional reconstructions of MD and BR muscles based on MEP recordings before and after training. The total variance accounted for by fitting the

    Lorentzian function to the data points is given by the R2 value. (B) Left, the diagrams illustrate the measure of the distance between MD and BR hot spots in one subject chosen

    to illustrate the maximal reduction of the inter-spot distances. Right, the histograms represent the differences between inter-spot distances before and after training in the

    two conditions for the six subjects.

    F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162 159

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    8/10

    volunteers, each studied on two occasions several weeks apart

    (ranging from 21 to 132 days) and showed a low degree of variabil-

    ity of repetitive TMS (rTMS) effects in the human brain. The areas

    of the scalp from which responses were evoked from corticospinal

    cells projecting to three intrinsic hand muscles were systematically

    mapped using TMS on eight normal subjects at different intervals

    (24 h, one week and two weeks). The area, volume, and centre

    of gravity of these maps did not change significantly over that per-

    iod. It was concluded that TMS mapping is suitable for studies that

    aim to examine the effect of various interventions on the cortical

    representation of individual muscles in human subjects (Miranda

    et al., 1997; Uy et al., 2002.; Pitcher et al., 2003; Monfils et al.,

    2005). Variation in the response to TMS in the general population

    allowed Wassermann (2002) to state with confidence that the MEP

    to single and paired TMS is a sensitive measurement of the cortico-

    spinal function. Taken together, and even though some differences

    in aMT and cortical excitability exist between subjects, these find-

    ings demonstrate that cortical maps are reproducible in a given

    subject over long periods of time and are similar in both hemi-

    spheres, and are similar in left- and right-handed subjects. The sta-

    bility of aMT observed in our experiments between the twosessions validates our choice to consider the first session of mea-

    surements before training as the control condition, as well as our

    use of map parameters for investigation of long-term effects of

    training.

    4.4. A cortical representation of coordination?

    The capacity of M1 to undergo plastic reorganization following

    skill acquisition and training is well established in humans and

    animals (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994, 1995; Classen et al., 1998; Kar-

    ni et al., 1998; Kleim et al., 1998; Liepert et al., 2000; Plautz et al.,

    2000; Yahagi and Kasai, 1998; Muellbacher et al., 2001; Tyc and

    Boyadjian, 2006.). Many studies have investigated the effect ofshort-term training on the cortical representation of distal muscles

    executing repetitive sequences of finger movements (Tyc and Boy-

    adjian, 2006). For example, in normal human subjects instructed to

    practice a repetitive sequence of finger movements, the amplitude

    and extent of finger muscle representation assessed by TMS in-

    creased in M1 contralateral to the performing hand as the speed

    of performance increased over a single day of training (Pascual-

    Leone et al., 1994). In contrast, we focus our experiments on the ef-

    fects of a six-week training period on the cortical representation of

    a complex coordinated movement namely the movement executed

    by a subject playing darts, in which all the joints in an upper limb

    are involved in a specific coordination pattern and timing of prox-

    imal and distal muscles (Temprado et al., 1997; Scott, 2000).

    Motor patterns at each joint must be carefully coordinated to

    smoothly move the joints and the hand through space. Many stud-

    ies have described the trajectories of hand and arm movements un-

    der normal conditions as straight hand paths called stereotyped

    movements (Bernstein, 1967; Flash and Hogan, 1985; Wolpert

    et al., 1995). These were defined as simple basic motor elements

    (primitives). The notion that skilled movements might reflect a

    process for the concatenation of consecutive movement elements

    into complex movements was suggested by Sosnik et al. (2004).

    On this assumption, a new primitive movement could be generated

    by training which would be different from the sum of the elements

    that comprised the sequence of initial movements (Engel et al.,

    1997). By studying the acquisition of skilled motor performance,

    Karni et al. (1995, 1998) showed that performance was paralleled

    by the emergence of a more extensive representation of a trained

    sequence of movements in M1. It has been suggested that a specificrepresentation of the sequence might be implemented at the M1

    level as a new functional unit, differentially from the component

    movements (Karni et al., 1998). In monkeys, practice-dependent

    changes in the functional topography of M1 were reported by Nudo

    Table 3

    Distances (mm) between estimated hot spot of MD and BR muscles before and after

    training in the two conditions (Alone and Co-active) for the six subjects. These data

    were used to measure the differences between inter-spot distances represented in

    Fig. 4B right.

    Distance between MD and BR hot spot

    Alone Condition Co-Active Condition

    Before After Before After

    1 20.0 18.1 22.7 14.2

    2 18.0 4.1 7.5 0.5

    3 10.4 11.5 11.7 11.3

    4 15.3 9.8 14.2 11.3

    5 15.5 7.2 15.3 10.4

    6 15.3 13 13.2 8.8

    Mean 15.7 10.6 14.1 9.4

    Fig. 5. (A) Excitability curves obtained on the BR muscle hot spot for one subject before and after training in the two conditions Alone (straight line) and Co-Active (dotted

    line). (B) Histograms representing the values of the plateau obtained on the excitability curves before and after training in the two conditions for the six subjects. Note the

    different scale for subject number 3 on the right (ordinates). * indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05) in the plateaus reached in the Alone and Co-Active conditions;** indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05) in the plateaus reached before training and after training.

    160 F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    9/10

    et al. (1996, 2001). After a few weeks of training on a task that re-

    quired skilled manipulation, a new movement representation was

    observed in M1 that was not found in untrained brains. These find-

    ings support the idea of a cortical representation of the learned

    motor sequence in M1 and underpinned our experimental

    protocol.

    Our finding that the co-activation of the proximal and the distal

    muscles enhanced the TMS-evoked motor output in the distal BRmuscle showed that some functional networks were triggered dur-

    ing coordinated movements. We interpreted the enlargement of

    the motor cortical output maps to the muscles involved in the task

    and their overlap indicative of a functional link between the two

    target muscles carrying out the coordination. Our observation that

    the proximal muscle modified the excitability curves of the distal

    muscle confirmed that the sharing of networks is necessary for

    coordination, as observed in other studies (Devanne et al., 2002;

    Ginnaneschi et al., 2006).

    TMS studies have shown that the cortical representations of dif-

    ferent muscles overlap on M1 (Wilson et al., 1993; Wassermann

    et al., 1993; Liepert et al., 1999; Tyc et al., 2005; Devanne et al.,

    2006). In animals, studies involving the superposition of M1 topo-

    graphical maps obtained by micro-stimulation, have shown that

    motor cortical zones controlling different forelimb segments are

    interconnected by intrinsic horizontal collaterals (Capaday et al.,

    1998; Huntley and Jones, 1991; Keller, 1993; Rioult-Pedotti and

    Donoghue, 2003; Tokuno and Tanji, 1993). In Rhesus monkeys,

    Park et al. (2001) found regions combining both distal and proxi-

    mal muscles following electrical stimulation of the cortical surface.

    In the cat, simultaneous stimulations of two cortical points were

    additive and resulted in a new movement that was a combination

    of the movements elicited by stimulation of each point on its own

    (Ethier et al., 2006). Brain imaging or TMS have shown that arm or

    hand movements were triggered by large areas of the motor cortex

    (Amassian et al., 1995; Devanne et al., 2002; Hallet, 2000; Kobay-

    ashi and Pascual-Leone, 2003; Sanes et al., 1995; Schieber and Hib-

    bard, 1993; Walsh and Cowey, 2000 Schieber, 2001). These

    findings suggest that widely distributed motor cortical territoriesare functionally linked during coordinated movements. Whether

    the expansion of the muscle representations in M1 observed in

    the darts players can be related to the development of a functional

    unit remains a matter of speculation. However, the highly distrib-

    uted organization of M1, convergence, divergence and horizontal

    connections with wide overlapping territories of different muscles,

    makes the hypothesis of a cortical representation of movement

    coordination plausible.

    Acknowledgements

    We thank our students for giving their time to participate in

    this study. We thank Professor A.R. Lieberman for editing our man-

    uscript and correcting the English language. We are grateful to Pro-

    fessor John C. Rothwell for his advice.

    References

    Ackerley SJ, Stinear CM, Byblow WD. The effect of coordination mode on use-dependent plasticity. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:175966.

    Amassian VE, Cracco RQ, Maccabee PJ, Cracco JB, Henry K. Some positive effects oftranscranial magnetic stimulation. In: Fahn S, Hallett M, Lders HO, MarsdenCD, editors. Adv Neurol: Negative motor phenomena 1995;vol.67. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven Publishers; 1995. p. 79106.

    Baylor AM, Benjuya N. H-reflexes during a motor skill acquisition task. Percept MotSkills. 1989;69:65970.

    Bernstein N. The coordination and regulation of movement. Pergamon, Oxford.1967.

    Capaday C, Devanne H, Bertrand L, Lavoie BA. Intracortical connections between

    motor cortical zones controlling antagonistic muscles in the cat: a combinedanatomical and physiological study. Exp Brain Res 1998;120:22332.

    Classen J, Liepert J, Wise SP, Hallet M, Cohen LG. Rapid plasticity of human corticalmovement representation induced by practice. J Neurophysiol1998;79:111723.

    Day BL, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Dick JP, Cowan JM. Berardelli et al. Motor cortexstimulation in intact man: 2. multiple descending volleys. Brain1987a;110:1191209.

    Day BL, Thompson PD, Dick JP, Nakashima K, Marsden CD. Different sites of action ofelectrical and magnetic stimulation of the human brain. Neurosci Lett1987b;75:1016.

    Devanne H, Cohen LG, Kouchtir-Devanne N, Capaday C. Integrated motor cortical

    control of task-related muscles during pointing in humans. J Neurophysiol2002;87:300617.

    Devanne H, Cassim F, Ethier C, Brizzi L, Thevenon A, Capaday C. The comparable sizeand overlapping nature of upper limb distal and proximal musclerepresentations in the human motor cortex. Eur J Neurosci 2006;23:246776.

    Engel KC, Flanders M, Soechting JF. Anticipatory and sequential motor control inpiano playing. Exp Brain Res 1997;113:18999.

    Ethier C, Brizzi L, Darling WG, Capaday C. Linear summation of cat motor cortexoutputs. J Neurosci 2006;26:557481.

    Flash T, Hogan N. The coordination of arm movements: An experimentallyconfirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci 1985;5:1688703.

    Garry MI, Kamen G, Nordstrom MA. Hemispheric differences in the relationshipbetween corticomotor excitability changes following a fine-motor task andmotor learning. J Neurophysiol 2004;91:15708.

    Ginanneschi F, Del Santo F, Dominici F, Gelli F, Mazzocchio R, Rossi A. Changes incorticomotor excitability of hand muscles in relation to static shoulderpositions. Exp Brain Res 2005;161:37482.

    Ginanneschi F, Dominici F, Biasella A, Gelli F, Rossi A. Change in corticomotorexcitability of forearm muscles in relation to static shoulder positions. Brain Res2006;19731974:3328.

    Hallett M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature2000;406:14750.

    Huntley GW, Jones EG. Relationship of intrinsic connections to forelimb movementrepresentations in monkey motor cortex: a correlative anatomic andphysiological study. J Neurophysiol 1991;66:390413.

    Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams M, Turner R, Ungerleider LG. Functional MRIevidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature1995;377:1558.

    Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, et al. Theacquisition of skilled motor performance. fast and slow experience-drivenchanges in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:8618.

    Keller A. Intrinsic connections between representation zones in the cat motorcortex. Neuroreport 1993;4:5158.

    Kleim JA, Barbay S, Nudo RJ. Functional reorganization of the rat motor cortexfollowing motor skill learning. J Neurophysiol 1998;80:33215.

    Kobayashi M, Pascual-Leone A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in neurology.Lancet Neurol 2003;2:14556.

    Krutky M, Perreault EJ. Motor cortical measures of use-dependent plasticity aregraded from distal to proximal in the human upper limb. J Neurophysiol2007;98:323041.

    Levy WJ, Amassian VE, Schmid UD, Jungreis C. Mapping of motor cortex gyral sitesnon-invasively by transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal subjects andpatients. In: Levy WJ, Cracco RQ, Barker AT, Rothwell JC, editors. Magneticmotor stimulation: Basic principles and clinical experience. Electroenceph clinneurophysiol 1991;Suppl. 43. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1991. p. 5175.

    Liepert J, Terborg C, Weiller C. Motor plasticity induced by synchronized thumb andfoot movements. Exp Brain Res 1999;125:4359.

    Liepert J, Graef S, Uhde I, Leidner O, Weiller C. Training induced changes of motorcortex representations in stroke patients. Acta Neurol Scand 2000;101:3216.

    Maeda F, Gangitano M, Thall M, Pascual-Leone A. Inter- and intra-individualvariability of paired-pulse curves with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS).Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113:37682.

    Miranda PC, de Carvalho M, Conceicao I, Luis ML, Ducla-Soares E. A new method forreproducible coil positioning in transcranial magnetic stimulation mapping.Electoencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1997;105:11623.

    Monfils M-H, Plautz EJ, Kleim JA. In search of the motor engram: motor map

    plasticity as a mechanism for encoding motor experience. Neuroscientist2005;11:47183.

    Mortifee P, Stewart H, Schulzer M, Eisen A. Reliability of transcranial magneticstimulation for mapping the human motor cortex. Electroencephalogr ClinNeurophysiol 1994;93:1317.

    Muellbacher W, Ziemann U, Boroojerdi B, Cohen L, Hallett M. Role of the humanmotor cortex in rapid motor learning. Exp Brain Res 2001;136:4318.

    Nudo RJ, Milliken GW, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Use-dependent alterations inprimary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci 2001;16:785807.

    Nudo RJ, Milliken GW, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Use-dependent alterations ofmovement representation in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. JNeurosci 1996;16:785807.

    Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97113.

    Park MC, Belhaj-Saf A, Gordon M, Cheney PD. Consistent features in the forelimbrepresentation of primary motor cortex in rhesus macaques. J Neurosci2001;21:278492.

    Pascual-Leone A, Grafman J, Hallett M. Modulation of cortical motor output maps

    during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science1994;263:12879.

    F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian/ Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162 161

  • 8/22/2019 La Plasticidad de La Corteza Motora Es Inducida Mediante Coordinacion y Entrenamiento 2011

    10/10

    Pascual-Leone A, Dang N, Cohen LG, Brasil-Neto JP, Cammarota A, Hallett M.Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulationduring acquisition of new fine motor skills. J Neurophysiol 1995;74:103745.

    Penfield W, Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebralcortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 1937;60:389443.

    Penfield W, Rasmussen T, editors. The cerebral cortex of man. New York: TheMacmillan Co.; 1950.

    Perez MA, Bjarke KS, Lungholt KN, Nielson JB. Motor skill training induces changesin the excitability of the leg cortical area in healthy humans. Exp Brain Res2004;159:197205.

    Pitcher JB, Kirstin MO, Miles TS. Age and sex differences in human motor cortexinput-output characteristics. J Physiol 2003;546:60513.

    Plautz EJ, Milliken GW, Nudo RJ. Effects of repetitive motor training on movementrepresentations in adult squirrel monkeys: role of use versus learning.Neurobiol Learn Mem 2000;74:2755.

    Press WH, Flannery BP, Teukolsky SA, Vetterling WT. Numerical Recipes. 1986Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (UK).

    Ridding MC, Rothwell JC. Stimulus/response curves as a method of measuring motorcortical excitability in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol1997;105:3404.

    Rioult-Pedotti M-S, Donoghue JP. The nature and mechanisms of plasticity. In:Boniface S, Ziemann U, editors. Plasticity in the human nervous system.Investigation with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press; 2003. p. 125.

    Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Day BL, Dick JPR, Kachi T, Cowan JMA, et al. Motor cortexstimulation in intact man. 1 general characteristics of EMG responses indifferent muscles. Brain 1987;110:117390.

    Rothwell JC. Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation ofthe human cortex. J Neurosci Meth 1997;74:11322.

    Sanes JN, Donoghue JP, Thangaraj V, Edelman RR, Warach S. Shared neuralsubstrates controlling hand movements in human motor cortex. Science1995;268:17757.

    Schieber MH, Hibbard LS. How somatotopic is the motor cortex hand area? Science1993;261:48992.

    Schieber MH. Constraints on somatotopic organization in the primary motor cortex.J Neurophysiol 2001;86:212543.

    Scott SH. Role of motor cortex in coordinating multi-joint movements: is it time fora new paradigm? Can J Physiol Pharmacol 2000;78:92333.

    Sosnik R, Hauptmann B, Karni A, Flash T. When practice leads to co-articulation: theevolution of geometrically defined movement primitives. Exp. Brain Res2004;156:42238.

    Temprado J, Della-grasta M, Farrell M, Laurent M. A novice-expert comparison of(intralimb) coordination subserving the volleyball serve. Hum Mov Sci1997;16:65376.

    Tokuno H, Tanji J. Input organization of distal and proximal forelimb areas in themonkey primary motor cortex: a retrograde double labeling study. J CompNeurol 1993;333:199209.

    Tyc F, Boyadjian A, Devanne H. Motor cortex plasticity induced by extensivetraining revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation in human. Eur J Neurosci2005;21:25966.

    Tyc F, Boyadjian A. Cortical plasticity and motor activity studied with transcranial

    magnetic stimulation. Rev Neurosci 2006;17:46995.Tyc F, Boyadjian A. Modification of the cortical representations of distal and

    proximal muscles induced by training to a throwing task in human. 6th FENS(Forum of European Neuroscience), 2008 Geneva.

    Uy J, Ridding MC, Miles TS. Stability of maps of human motor cortex made withtranscranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topogr 2002;14:2937.

    Walsh V, Cowey A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience.Nat Rev Neurosci 2000;1:739.

    Wandell BA, Smirnakis SM. Plasticity and stability of visual field maps in adultprimary visual cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10(12):87384.

    Wassermann EM, McShane LM, Hallett M, Cohen L. Noninvasive mapping of musclerepresentations in human motor cortex. Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol1992;85:18.

    Wassermann EM, Pascual-Leone A, Valls-Sol J, Toro C, Cohen LG, Hallett M.Topography of the inhibitory and excitatory responses to transcranial magneticstimulation in a hand muscle. Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol1993;89:42433.

    Wassermann EM. Variation in the response to transcranial magnetic brainstimulation in the general population. Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113:116571.

    Wilson SA, Thickbroom GW, Mastaglia FL. Transcranial magnetic stimulationmapping of the motor cortex in normal subjects. J Neurol Sci 1993;118:13444.

    Wilson SA, Thickbroom GW, Mastaglia FL. Comparison of the magnetically mappedcorticomotor representation of a muscle at rest and during low-level voluntarycontraction. Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol 1995;97:24650.

    Wolpaw JR, Carp JS. Adaptative plasticity in spinal cord. Adv Neurol1993;59:16374.

    Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI. Are arms trajectories planned in kinematicor dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study. Exp Brain Res 1995;103:46070.

    Yahagi S, Kasai T. Facilitation of motor evoked potentials (MEPs) in first dorsalinterosseous (FDI) muscle is dependent on different motor images.Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol 1998;109:40917.

    162 F. Tyc, A. Boyadjian / Clinical Neurophysiology 122 (2011) 153162